Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

, Volume 63, Issue 5, pp 649–659 | Cite as

Fissioning minimizes ranging costs in spider monkeys: a multiple-level approach

  • Norberto Asensio
  • Amanda H. Korstjens
  • Filippo Aureli
Original Paper

Abstract

The adjustment to deal with intragroup food competition is probably the most plausible explanation of high levels of fission–fusion dynamics. However, studies did not always support expected relations between food availability, ranging costs, and subgroup size. We used several levels of analysis differing in the time and spatial scale in order to investigate this explanation in spider monkeys. In our study, subgroups were larger when food availability was higher across most levels of analyses used. We also found a fine-scale adjustment: compared to the food patch previously visited, spider monkeys traveled to larger patches just after fusions. This was not without an immediate travel cost: the interpatch distance and travel time after a fusion were longer than that before the fusion. This rapid adjustment shows the flexibility that fission–fusion dynamics can offer. Spider monkeys are in large subgroups only when food conditions are favorable, as evidenced by the fact that at all the other time-scale levels larger subgroups did not experience greater ranging costs than smaller subgroups. Our results indicate that on the whole spider monkeys successfully minimize ranging costs by fission and fusion of subgroups.

Keywords

Spider monkeys Fission–fusion Feeding competition Ranging costs 

References

  1. Alexander RD (1974) The evolution of social behaviour. Ann Rev Ecolog Syst 5:325–383CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Anderson DP, Nordheim EV, Boesch C, Moermond TC (2002) Factors influencing fission–fusion grouping in chimpanzees in the Tai National Park, Cote d’Ivoire. In: Boesch C, Hohmann G, Marchant LF (eds) Behavioural diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 90–101Google Scholar
  3. Archie EA, Morrison TA, Foley CAH, Moss CJ, Alberts SC (2006) Dominance rank relationships among wild female African elephants (Loxodonta africana). Anim Behav 71:117–127CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Asensio N, Korstjens AH, Schaffner CM, Aureli F (2008) Intragroup aggression, feeding competition and fission–fusion sociality in spider monkeys. Behaviour 145(7):983–1001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Aureli F, Schaffner CM, Boesch C, Bearder SK, Call J, Chapman CA, Connor R, Di Fiore A, Dunbar RIM, Henzi PS, Holekamp K, Korstjens AH, Layton R, Lee P, Lehmann J, Manson JH, Ramos-Fernandez G, Strier KB, van Schaik CP (2008) Fission–fusion dynamics: new research frameworks. Cur Anthropol 48:627–654CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Basabose AK (2004) Fruit availability and chimpanzee party size at Kahuzi Montane Forest, Democratic Republic of Congo. Primates 45(4):211–219PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Carbone C, Cowlishaw G, Isaac NJB, Rowcliffe J (2005) How far do animals go? Determinants of day range in mammals. Am Nat 165(2):290–297PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Chapman CA (1988) Patterns of foraging and range use by three species of neotropical primates. Primates 29:177–194CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Chapman CA (1990) Association patterns of male and female spider monkeys: the influence of ecology and sex on social organization. Behav Ecol Soc 26:409–414Google Scholar
  10. Chapman CA, Chapman LJ (2000) Determinants of group size in primates: the importance of travel costs. In: Boinski S, Garber PA (eds) On the move: how and why animals travel in groups. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 24–42Google Scholar
  11. Chapman CA, Wrangham W (1994) Indices of habitat-wide abundance in tropical forests. Biotropica 26(2):160–171CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Chapman CA, Chapman LJ, Wrangham W, Hunt D, Gebo D, Gardner L (1992) Estimators of fruit abundance of tropical trees. Biotropica 24:527–531CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Chapman CA, Wrangham RW, Chapman LJ (1995) Ecological constraints on group size: an analysis of spider monkey and chimpanzee subgroups. Behav Ecol Soc 36(1):59–70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Clutton-Brock TH, Harvey PH (1977) Primate ecology and social organization. J Zool 183:1–39CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Connor RC, Wells RS, Mann J, Read AJ (2000) The bottlenose dolphin, social relationships in a fission–fusion society. In: Mann J, Connor RC, Tyack PL, Whitehead H (eds) Cetacean societies: field studies of dolphins and whales. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 91–126Google Scholar
  16. Di Fiore A, Campbell CJ (2007) The atelines: variation in ecology, behavior, and social organization. In: Campbell A, Fuentes KC, Mackinnon KC, Panger M, Beader SK (eds) Primates in perspective. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 155–185Google Scholar
  17. Dunbar RIM (1988) Primate social systems. Cornell University Press, IthacaGoogle Scholar
  18. Ghiglieri MP (1984) Feeding ecology and sociality of chimpanzees in Kibale Forest, Uganda. In: Rodman PS, Cant JGH (eds) Adaptations for foraging in nonhuman primates: contributions to an organismal biology of prosimians, monkeys, and apes. Columbia University Press, New York, pp 161–194Google Scholar
  19. Hashimoto C, Suzuki S, Takenoshita Y, Yamagiwa J, Basabose AK, Furuichi T (2003) How fruit abundance affects the chimpanzee party size: a comparison between four study sites. Primates 44(2):77–81PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Heithaus MR, Dill LM (2002) Food availability and tiger shark predation risk influence bottlenose dolphin habitat use. Ecology 83(2):480–491CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Isbell LA (1991) Contest and scramble competition: patterns of female aggression and ranging behavior among primates. Behav Ecol 2(2):143–155CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Itoh N, Nishida T (2007) Chimpanzee grouping patterns and food availability in Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Primates 48:87–96PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Janson CH (1988) Intra-specific food competition and primate social structure: a synthesis. Behaviour 105(1–2):1–17CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Janson CH, Goldsmith ML (1995) Predicting group size in primates: foraging costs and predation risks. Behav Ecol 6(3):326–336CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Janson CH, van Schaik CP (1988) Recognizing the many faces of primate food competition: methods. Behaviour 105(1–2):165–186CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Janzen DH (1986) Guanacaste National Park: ecological and cultural restoration. UNED, San JoseGoogle Scholar
  27. Klein LL, Klein DB (1977) Feeding behaviour of the Colombian spider monkey. In: Clutton-Brock TH (ed) Primate ecology: studies of feeding and ranging behaviour in lemurs, monkeys and apes. Academic, New York, pp 153–181Google Scholar
  28. Koenig A (2002) Competition for resources and its behavioral consequences among female primates. Int J Primatol 23(4):759–783CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Korstjens AH, Verhoeckx IL, Dunbar RIM (2006) Time as a constraint on group size in spider monkeys. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 60:683–669CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Kruuk H (1972) The spotted hyena: a study of predation and social behavior. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  31. Kummer H (1971) Primate societies: group techniques of ecological adaptation. Aldine-Atherton, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  32. Lehmann J, Boesch C (2004) To fission or to fusion: effects of community size on wild chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes verus) social organisation. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 56(3):207–216CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Lehmann J, Korstjens AH, Dunbar RIM (2007) Fission–fusion social systems as a strategy for coping with ecological constraints: a primate case. Evol Ecol 21:613–634CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Lomnicki A (1988) Population ecology of individuals, monograph in population biology. Princeton University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  35. Mitani JC, Watts DP, Lwanga JS (2002) Ecological and social correlates of chimpanzee party size and composition. In: Boesch C, Hohmann G, Marchant LF (eds) Behavioural diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 102–111Google Scholar
  36. Muller MN (2002) Agonistic relations among Kanyawara chimpanzees. In: Boesch C, Hohmann G, Marchant LF (eds) Behavioural diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 112–124Google Scholar
  37. Newton-Fisher NE, Reynolds V, Plumptre AJ (2000) Food supply and chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) party size in the Budongo Forest Reserve, Uganda. Int J Primatol 21(4):613–628CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Nicholson AJ (1954) Compensatory reactions of populations to stresses, and their evolutionary significance. Aust J Zool 2:1–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Nishida T (1968) The social group of wild chimpanzees in the Mahali mountains. Primates 9:167–224CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Ramos-Fernandez G, Boyer D, Gomez VP (2006) A complex social structure with fission–fusion properties can emerge from a simple foraging model. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 60(4):536–549CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Siegel S, Castellan NJ (1988) Nonparametric statistics for the behavioural sciences. McGraw-Hill, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  42. Smith JE, Kolowski JM, Graham SED, Holekamp KE (2008) Social and ecological determinants of fission–fusion dynamics in the spotted hyena. Anim Behav 76:619–636CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Stephen DW, Krebs JR (1986) Monographs in behavior ecology. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  44. Sterck EHM, Watts DP, van Schaik CP (1997) The evolution of female social relationships in nonhuman primates. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 41(5):291–309CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Stevenson PR, Quinones MJ, Ahumada JA (1998) Effects of fruit patch availability on feeding subgroup size and spacing patterns in four primate species at Tinigua National Park, Colombia. Int J Primatol 19(2):313–324CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Sugiyama Y (1999) Socioecological factors of male chimpanzee migration at Bossou, Guinea. Primates 40(1):61–68CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Symington MM (1988) Food competition and foraging party size in the black spider monkey (Ateles paniscus chamek). Behaviour 105(1–2):117–134Google Scholar
  48. Symington MM (1990) Fission–fusion social organization in Ateles and Pan. Int J Primatol 11(1):47–61CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. te Boekhorst IJA, Schurmann CL, Sugardjito J (1990) Residential status and seasonal movements of wild orangutans in the Gunung Leuser Reserve (Sumatera, Indonesia). Anim Behav 39:1098–1109CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Terborgh J, Janson CH (1986) The socioecology of primate groups. Ann Rev Ecol Syst 17:111–136CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. van Schaik CP (1999) The socioecology of fission–fusion sociality in orangutans. Primates 40(1):69–86CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. van Schaik CP, van Hooff JARAM (1983) On the ultimate causes of primate social systems. Behaviour 85:91–117CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Vogel ER, Janson CH (2007) Predicting the frequency of food-related agonism in white-faced capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus), using a novel focal-tree method. Am J Primatol 69:533–560PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Wallace RB (2008) The influence of feeding patch size and relative fruit density on the foraging behaviour of the black spider monkey Ateles chamek. Biotropica 40(4):501–506CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Williams JM, Liu HY, Pusey AE (2002) Costs and benefits of grouping for female chimpanzees at Gombe. In: Boesch C, Hohmann G, Marchant LF (eds) Behavioural diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 192–203Google Scholar
  56. Wrangham RW (1979) On the evolution of ape social systems. Soc Sci Inf 18(3):335–368CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  • Norberto Asensio
    • 1
  • Amanda H. Korstjens
    • 2
  • Filippo Aureli
    • 1
  1. 1.Research Center in Evolutionary Anthropology and Paleoecology, School of Natural Sciences and PsychologyLiverpool John Moores UniversityLiverpoolUK
  2. 2.School of Conservation SciencesBournemouth UniversityBournemouthUK

Personalised recommendations