Skip to main content
Log in

Determinants of vigilance behavior in the ring-tailed coati (Nasua nasua): the importance of within-group spatial position

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Individuals living in social groups are predicted to live under unequal predation risk due to their spatial location within the group. Previous work has indicated that individuals located at the edge of groups have higher “domains of danger”, thus are more likely to engage in vigilance or antipredator behavior. We studied the determinants of vigilance behavior in two groups of ring-tailed coatis in Iguazu National Park, Argentina. In addition to the expected pattern that coatis were more vigilant at the edge of the group, we found that individuals were particularly vigilant at the front edge of the group. This pattern conforms to predictions of differing predation risk caused by sit-and-wait predators with respect to mobile animal groups. In addition, coatis exhibited less vigilance when the number of neighbors within 5 m and group size increased. Of the three spatial variables tested, within-group spatial position was the most important predictor variable determining vigilance levels. These results confirm that spatial position has major effects on vigilance behavior, and that group directionality is an important factor which should be taken into account when measuring vigilance behavior. Coatis were more vigilant when juveniles less than 6 months old were in the groups. The presence of these young juveniles also affected the relationship between alarm response and vigilance levels. Coatis were more vigilant after strong alarm reactions, but only when young juveniles were not present in the groups. This may indicate that coatis give differential responses to alarm calls depending on the age of the caller. A comparison of antipredator vigilance between coatis and sympatric capuchin monkeys is consistent with the hypothesis that terrestriality leads to higher perceive predation risk for coatis.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Beauchamp G (2001) Should vigilance always decrease with group size? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 51:47–52

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beauchamp G (2003) Group-size effects on vigilance: a search for mechanisms. Behav Processes 63:111–121

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Black JM, Carbone C, Wells RL, Owen M (1992) Foraging dynamics in goose flocks—the cost-of-living on the edge. Anim Behav 44:41–50

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blumstein DT, Daniel JC (2004) Yellow-bellied marmots discriminate among the alarm calls of individuals and are more responsive to the calls from pups. Anim Behav 68:1257–1265

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blumstein DT, Daniel JC, Evans CS (2001a) Yellow-footed rock-wallaby group size effects reflect a trade-off. Ethology 107:655–664

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blumstein DT, Daniel JC, McLean IG (2001b) Group size effects in quokkas. Aust J Zool 49:641–649

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boinski S, Kauffman L, Westoll A, Stickler CM, Crop S, Ehmke E (2003) Are vigilance, risk from avian predators and group size consequences of habitat structure? A comparison of three species of squirrel monkey (Saimiri oerstedii, S. boliviensis, and S. sciureus). Behaviour 140:1421–1467

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown AD, Zunino GE (1990) Dietary variability in Cebus apella in extreme habitats: evidence for adaptability. Folia Primatol 54:187–195

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bumann D, Krause J, Rubenstein D (1997) Mortality risk of spatial positions in animal groups: the danger of being in the front. Behaviour 134:1063–1076

    Google Scholar 

  • Burger J (2001) Visibility, group size, vigilance, and drinking behavior in coati (Nasua narica) and white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus): experimental evidence. Acta Ethologica 3:111–119

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burger J, Gochfeld M (1992) Effect of group size on vigilance while drinking in the coati, Nasua narica in Costa Rica. Anim Behav 44:1053–1057

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caro T (2005) Antipredator defenses in birds and mammals. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark CW, Dukas R (2003) The behavioral ecology of a cognitive constraint: limited attention. Behav Ecol 14:151–156

    Google Scholar 

  • Coolen I, Giraldeau LA, Lavoie M (2001) Head position as an indicator of producer and scrounger tactics in a ground feeding bird. Anim Behav 61:895–903

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cords M (1990) Vigilance and mixed-species association of some East-African forest monkeys. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 26:297–300

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cresswell W, Lind J, Kaby U, Quinn JL, Jakobsson S (2003) Does an opportunistic predator preferentially attack nonvigilant prey? Anim Behav 66:643–648 DOI:10.1006/anbe.2003.2233

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehn MM (1990) Vigilance for predators: detection and dilution effects. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 26:337–342

    Google Scholar 

  • Di Bitetti MS (2001a) Home range use by the tufter capuchin monkey (Cebus apella nigritus) in a subtropical rainforest of Argentina. J Zool 253:33–45

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Di Bitetti MS (2001b) Food-associated calls in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Ph.D.thesis, State University of New York at Stony Brook

  • Elgar MA (1989) Predator vigilance and group size in mammals and birds: a critical review of the empirical evidence. Biol Rev 64:13–33

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • FitzGibbon CD (1989) A cost to individuals with reduced vigilance in groups of Thomsons gazelles hunted by cheetahs. Anim Behav 37:508–510

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giraldeau LA, Caraco T (2000) Social foraging theory. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Gompper ME (1996) Sociality and asociality in white-nosed coatis (Nasua narica): foraging costs and benefits. Behav Ecol 7(3):254–263

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall CL, Fedigan LM (1997) Spatial benefits afforded by high rank in white-faced capuchins. Anim Behav 53:1069–1082

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton WD (1971) Geometry for the selfish herd. J Theor Biol 31:295–311

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hass CC, Valenzuela D (2002) Anti-Predator benefits of group living in white-nosed coatis (Nasua narica). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 51:570–578

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirsch BT (2002) Social monitoring and vigilance behavior in brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 52:458–464

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inglis IR, Lazarus J (1981) Vigilance and flock size in brent gees: the edge effect. Z Tierpsychol 57:193–200

    Google Scholar 

  • Janson CH (1990a) Ecological consequences of individual spatial choice in foraging groups of brown capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella. Anim Behav 40:922–934

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Janson CH (1990b) Social correlates of individual spatial choice in foraging groups of brown capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella. Anim Behav 40:910–921

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Janson CH, Di Bitetti MS (1997) Experimental analysis of food detection in capuchin monkeys: effects of distance, travel speed, and resource size. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 41:17–24

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kramer DL, McLaughlin RL (2001) The behavioral ecology of intermittent locomotion. Am Zool 41:137–153

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krause J (1994) Differential fitness returns in relation to spatial position in groups. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 69:187–206

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krause J, Ruxton GD (2002) Living in groups. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Lima SL (1994) On the personal benefits of anti-predatory vigilance. Anim Behav 48:734–736

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin PR, Bateson P (1996) Measuring behaviour, 2nd edn. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • McCowen B, Franceshini NV, Vicino GA (2004) Age differences and developmental trends in alarm peep responses by squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). Am J Primatol 53:19–31

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Resende BD, Mannu M, Izar P, Ottoni EB (2004) Interaction between capuchins and coatis: nonagonistic behaviors and lack of predation. Int J Primatol 25:1213–1225

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rose LM (1998) Behavioral ecology of white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus) in Costa Rica. Ph.D. thesis, Washington University, St. Louis

  • Russell JK (1979) Reciprocity in the social behavior of coatis (Nasua narica). Ph.D. thesis, The University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill

  • Schoener TW (1981) An empirically based estimate of home range. Theor Popul Biol 20:281–325

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (1980) The ontogeny of vervet monkey alarm-calling behavior: a preliminary report. Z Tierpsychol 54:37–56

    Google Scholar 

  • Steenbeek R, Piek RC, van Buul M, van Hooff J (1999) Vigilance in wild Thomas’s langurs (Presbytis thomasi): the importance of infanticide risk. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 45:137–150

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor RJ (1988) Territory size and location in animals with refuges: influence of predation risk. Evol Ecol 2:95–101

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treves A (1998) The influence of group size and neighbors on vigilance in two species of arboreal monkeys. Behaviour 135:453–481

    Google Scholar 

  • Treves A (2000) Theory and method in studies of vigilance and aggregation. Anim Behav 60:711–722

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treves A, Drescher A, Ingrisano N (2001) Vigilance and aggregation in black howler monkeys (Alouatta pigra). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 50(1): 90–95

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vine I (1971) Risk of visual detection and pursuit by a predator and the selective advantage of flocking behavior. J Theor Biol 30:405–422

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Viscido SV, Millera M, Wethey DS (2001) The response of a selfish herd to an attack from outside the group perimeter. J Theor Biol 208:315–328 DOI: 10.1006/jtbi.2000.2221

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Santiago Escobar, Carolina Ferrari, and Fermino Silva for help and assistance during the course of the field work. We would also like to thank Viviana Muñoz for her veterinary assistance. The authors are particularly grateful to Charles Janson for the immeasurable amount of advice he gave us during all aspects of this project, and letting us borrow several pieces of much needed field equipment. The authors are also very thankful for the consistently helpful comments and advice from Mario Di Bitetti. We would like to thank Barry Hirsch for statistical advice and help using SAS. This paper has benefited tremendously, thanks to comments by Mario Di Bitetti, Dan Blumstein, Joanna Burger, Matt Gompper, Charles Janson, Roland Kays, and Abe Ruttenburg. We thank the APN for the permission to carry out work in Iguazu. This study was funded in part by a NSF DDIG grant.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ben T. Hirsch.

Additional information

Communicated by: P. Bednekoff

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Di Blanco, Y., Hirsch, B.T. Determinants of vigilance behavior in the ring-tailed coati (Nasua nasua): the importance of within-group spatial position. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 61, 173–182 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-006-0248-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-006-0248-3

Keywords

Navigation