Skip to main content
Log in

Does information of predators influence general wariness?

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Antipredator behavior includes several qualitatively distinct activities, but few studies have determined the degree to which these activities are independent. If behaviors are not independent, then the nature of the relationship would illustrate potential performance constraints. We studied crimson rosellas (Platycercus elegans) and first focused on acoustic predator discrimination. We measured time allocation before and after playback of one of three experimental treatments (peregrine falcons—Falco peregrinus, wedge-tailed eagles—Aquila audax, and crimson rosellas) to determine whether or not rosellas discriminated predators from non-predators, and specifically whether or not they discriminated large from small predators. We then focused on the decision to flee. We experimentally approached subjects and measured the distance at which they oriented to us (alert distance) and the distance at which they fled (flight initiation distance; FID). We found that rosellas could distinguish among predators; however, there was no effect on general wariness as measured by FID. These two processes of antipredator behavior may, thus, be independent.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Blumstein DT (2003) Flight-initiation distance in birds is dependent on intruder starting distance. J Wildl Manage 67:852–857

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blumstein DT, Fernández-Juricic E, LeDee O, Larson E, Rodriguez-Prieto I, Zugmeyer C (2004) Avian risk assessment: effects of perching height and detectability. Ethology 110:273–285

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blumstein DT, Fernández-Juricic E, Zoolner PA, Garity SC (2005) Inter-specific variation in avian responses to human disturbance. J Appl Ecol 42:943–953

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blumstein DT, Daniel JC, Evans CS (2006) JWatcher 1.0: An introductory user’s guide. http://www.jwatcher.ucla.edu

  • Buckingham R, Jackson L (1985) A field guide to birdsong, cassette 2: rufous night heron to chestnut rail. Bird Observers Club, Nunawading, Victoria

    Google Scholar 

  • Cárdenas YL, Shen B, Zung L, Blumstein DT (2005) Evaluating temporal and spatial margins of safety in galahs. Anim Behav 70:1395–1399

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caro T (2005) Antipredator defenses in birds and mammals. University of Chicago, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper WE (1997) Threat factors affecting antipredatory behavior in the broad-headed skink (Eumeces laticeps): repeated approach, change in predator path, and predator’s field of view. Copeia 3:613–619

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deecke VB, Slater PJB, Ford JKB (2002) Selective habituation shapes acoustic predator recognition in harbour seals. Nature 420:171–173

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dial KP (2003) Evolution of avian locomotion: correlates of flight style, locomotor modules, nesting biology, body size, development, and the origin of flapping flight. Auk 120:941–952

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fichtel C, Kappeler PM (2002) Anti-predator behavior of group-living Malagasy primates: mixed evidence for a referential alarm call system. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 51:262–275

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gil-da-Costa R, Palleroni A, Hauser MD, Touchton J, Kelley J (2003) Rapid acquisition of an alarm response by a neotropical primate to a newly introduced avian predator. Proc R Soc London, Ser B 270:605–610

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grossman ML, Hamlet J (1988) Birds of prey of the world. Crown Publishers, New York, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Harder M (2000) Diet and breeding biology of the wedge-tailed eagle (Aquila audax) at three nest sites in north-eastern New South Wales. Corella 24:1–5

    Google Scholar 

  • Hakkarainen H, Yli-Tuomi I, Korpimäki E, Ydenberg R (2002) Provisioning response to manipulation of apparent predation danger by parental pied flycatchers. Ornis Fenn 79:139–144

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser MD, Caffrey C (1994) Anti-predator response to raptor calls in wild crows, Corvus brachyrhynchos hesperis. Anim Behav 48:1469–1471

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hodos W, Erichsen JT (1990) Lower-field myopia in birds: an adaptation that keeps the ground in focus. Vision Res 30:653–658

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Krebs EA (1998) Breeding biology of crimson rosellas (Platycerus elegans) on Black Mountain, Australian Capital Territory. Aust J Zool 46:119–136

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lank DB, Ydenberg RC (2003) Death and danger at migratory stopovers: problems with “predation risk”. J Avian Biol 34:225–228

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lima SL, Bednekoff PA (1999) Back to the basics of antipredatory vigilance: cannonvigilant animals detect attack? Anim Behav 58:537–543

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lima SL, Dill LM (1990) Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Can J Zool 68:619–640

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin P, Bateson P (1986) Measuring behaviour: an introductory guide. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Olsen J, Debus S, Rose AB, Hayes G (2004) Breeding success, cliff characteristics of peregrine falcon at high altitude in Australian Capital Territory. Corella 28:33–37

    Google Scholar 

  • Palleroni A, Hauser M, Marler P (2005) Do responses of galliform birds vary adaptively with predator size? Anim Cognit 8:200–210

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pizzey G (1980) A field guide to the birds of Australia. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Rainey HJ, Zuberbühler K, Slater PJB (2004) The responses of black-casqued hornbills to predator vocalisations and primate alarm calls. Behaviour 141:1263–1277

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Searcy YM, Caine NG (2003) Hawk calls elicit alarm and defensive reactions in captive Geoffroy’s marmosets (Callithrix geoffroiyi). Folia Primatol 74:115-125

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sharp A, Gibson L, Norton M, Ryan B, Marks A, Semararo L (2002) Breeding season diet of the wedge-tailed eagle (Aquila audax) in western New South Wales and influence of rabbit calicivirus disease. Wildl Res 29:175-184

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stubbs T (1996) http://users.tpg.com.au/users/vanh/4.4.1_variety.html

  • Swaisgood RR, Rowe MP, Owings DH (1999) Assessment of rattlesnake dangerousness by California ground squirrels: exploitation of cues from rattling sounds. Anim Behav 57:1301-1310

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Templeton CN, Greene E, Davis K (2005) Allometry of alarm calls: black-capped chickadees encode information about predator size. Science 308:1934-1937

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tobalske BW, Dial KP (2000) Effects of body size on take-off flight performance in the Phasianidae (Aves). J Exp Biol 203:319-3332

    Google Scholar 

  • Ydenberg RC, Dill LM (1986) The economics of fleeing from predators. Adv Study Behav 16:224-229

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

For permission to work in Booderee National Park, we thank the National Park staff and the Wreck Bay Aboriginal Community. Additionally, we thank Tony Davidson for permission to work on the H.M.A.S. Creswell. We thank the UCLA Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, the Lida Scott Brown Ornithology Trust, and the UCLA Office of Instructional Development for their generous support. Special thanks go to Brian Smith for help in the field and the lab, Benjamin Theule for editorial help and insight, and Eve Buigues for support. We thank Peter Bednekoff and three anonymous reviewers for extremely constructive criticism on a previous version.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Daniel T. Blumstein.

Additional information

Communicated by P. Bednekoff

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Adams, J.L., Camelio, K.W., Orique, M.J. et al. Does information of predators influence general wariness?. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 60, 742–747 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-006-0218-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-006-0218-9

Keywords

Navigation