Skip to main content
Log in

The effect of perceived intruder proximity and resident body size on the aggressive responses of male green frogs, Rana clamitans (Anura: Ranidae)

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We examined variation in aggressive responses within and among individuals in the green frog, Rana clamitans. We tested the hypothesis that resident males respond in a graded fashion to changes in perceived intruder proximity (stimulus intensity). We also investigated how response level varied with responder body size. We found that green frogs differentially alter aggressive responses (increasing movement and calling rates while decreasing the duration and dominant frequency of their calls) with an increase in stimulus intensity. Body size did not appear to be a significant influence on most response levels. We suggest that aggressive responses are graded, and advertisement and aggressive calls represent opposite ends of a continuum. Green frogs do not exhibit clearly defined thresholds for responses like those reported for some other frogs. By using graded responses rather than discrete “all-or-nothing” responses, males may reduce the potential costs of aggressive encounters.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams ES, Mesterton-Gibbons M (1995) The cost of threat displays and the stability of deceptive communication. J Theor Biol 175:405–421

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amo L, López P, Martín J (2004) Chemosensory recognition and behavioral responses of wall lizards, Podarcis muralis, to scents of snakes that pose different risks of predation. Copeia 2004:691–696

    Google Scholar 

  • Andersson M (1994) Sexual selection. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Arak A (1983a) Sexual selection by male–male competition in natterjack toad choruses. Nature 306:261–262

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arak A (1983b) Vocal interactions, call matching and territoriality in a Sri Lankan treefrog, Philatus leucorhinus (Rhacophoridae). Anim Behav 31:292–302

    Google Scholar 

  • Archer J (1988) The behavioural biology of aggression. Cambridge University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Austin JD, Lougheed SC, Moler PE, Boag PT (2003) Phylogenetics, zoogeography, and the role of dispersal and vicariance in the evolution of the Rana catesbeiana (Anura: Ranidae) species group. Biol J Linn Soc 80:601–624

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bee MA, Bowling AC (2002) Socially mediated pitch alteration by territorial male bullfrogs, Rana catesbeiana. J Herpetol 36:140–143

    Google Scholar 

  • Bee MA, Kozich CE, Blackwell KJ, Gerhardt HC (2001) Individual variation in advertisement calls of territorial male green frogs, Rana clamitans: implications for individual discrimination. Ethology 107:65–84

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bee MA, Perrill SA (1996) Responses to conspecific advertisement calls in the green frog (Rana clamitans) and their role in male-male communication. Behaviour 133:283–301

    Google Scholar 

  • Bee MA, Perrill SA, Owen PC (1999) Size assessment in simulated territorial encounters between male green frogs (Rana clamitans). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 45:177–184

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bee MA, Perrill SA, Owen PC (2000) Male green frogs lower the pitch of acoustic signals in defense of territories: a possible dishonest signal of size? Behav Ecol 11:169–177

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bond AB (1989a) Toward a resolution of the paradox of aggressive displays: I. Optimal deceit in the communication of fighting ability. Ethology 81:29–46

    Google Scholar 

  • Bond AB (1989b) Toward a resolution of the paradox of aggressive displays: II. Behavioral efference and the communication of intentions. Ethology 81:235–249

    Google Scholar 

  • Bond AB (1992) Aggressive motivation in the Midas cichlid: evidence for behavioral efference. Behaviour 122:135–152

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradbury JW, Vehrencamp SL (1998) Principles of animal communication. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Brenowitz EA (1989) Neighbor call amplitude influences aggressive behavior and intermale spacing in choruses of the Pacific treefrog (Hyla regilla). Ethology 83:69–79

    Google Scholar 

  • Brenowitz EA, Rose GJ, Alder T (2001) The neuroethology of acoustic communication in Pacific treefrogs. In: Ryan MJ (ed) Anuran communication. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, pp 145–155

    Google Scholar 

  • Brzoska J, Schneider H, Nevo E (1982) Territorial behavior and vocal responses in male Hyla arborea savignyi (Amphibia: Anura). Israel J Zool 31:27–37

    Google Scholar 

  • Burmeister S, Wilczynski W, Ryan MJ (1999) Temporal call changes and prior experience affect graded signaling in the cricket frog. Anim Behav 57:611–618

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burmeister SS, Ophir AG, Ryan MJ, Wilczynski W (2002) Information transfer during cricket frog contests. Anim Behav 64:715–725

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christensen-Dalsgaard J, Ludwig TA, Narins PM (2002) Call diversity in an Old World treefrog: level dependence and latency of acoustic responses. Bioacoustics 13:21–35

    Google Scholar 

  • Davies NB, Halliday TR (1978) Deep croaks and fighting assessment in toads, Bufo bufo. Nature 274:683–685

    Google Scholar 

  • Dyson ML, Passmore NI (1992) Inter-male spacing and aggression in African painted reed frogs, Hyperolius marmoratus. Ethology 91:237–247

    Google Scholar 

  • Gatten RE Jr, Miller K, Full RJ (1992) Energetics at rest and during locomotion. In: Feder ME, Buggren WW (eds) Environmental physiology of the amphibians. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 314–377

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerhardt HC, Huber F (2002) Acoustic communication in insects and anurans. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Given MF (1987) Vocalizations and acoustic interactions of the carpenter frog, Rana virgatipes. Herpetologica 43:467–481

    Google Scholar 

  • Given MF (1999) Frequency alteration of the advertisement call in the carpenter frog, Rana virgatipes. Herpetologica 55:304–317

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon N (2004) The effect of supplemental feeding on the territorial behavior of the green frog, Rana clamitans. Amphibia-Reptilia 25:55–62

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grafe TU (1995) Graded aggressive calls in the African painted reed frog Hyperolius marmoratus (Hyperoliidae). Ethology 101:67–81

    Google Scholar 

  • Grafe TU (2003) Synchronized interdigitated calling in the Kuvangu running frog, Kassina kuvangensis. Anim Behav 66:127–136

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grafe TU, Steffen JO, Stoll C (2000) Vocal repertoire and effect of advertisement call intensity on calling behaviour in the West African tree frog, Leptopelis viridis. Amphibia-Reptilia 21:13–23

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grafen A (1990) Biological signals as handicaps. J Theor Biol 144:517–546

    CAS  PubMed  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  • Harrison PA, Littlejohn MJ (1985) Diphasy in the advertisement calls of Geocrinia laevis (Anura: Leptodactylidae): vocal responses of males during field playback experiments. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18:67–73

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser MD, Nelson DA (1991) “Intentional” signaling in animal communication. Trends Ecol Evol 6:186–189

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howard RD, Young JR (1998) Individual variation in male vocal traits and female mating preferences in Bufo americanus. Anim Behav 55:1165–1179

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hurd PL, Enquist M (2001) Threat display in birds. Can J Zool 79:931–942

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jehle R, Arak A (1998) Graded call variation in the Asian cricket frog Rana nicobariensis. Bioacoustics 9:35–48

    Google Scholar 

  • Jenssen T, Preston WB (1968) Behavioral responses of the male green frog, Rana clamitans, to its recorded call. Herpetologica 43:467–481

    Google Scholar 

  • Lange H, Leimar O (2003) The function of threat display in wintering great tits. Anim Behav 65:573–584

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laurilia A, Järvi-Laturi M, Pakkasmaa S., Merilä J (2004) Temporal variation in predation risk: stage-dependency, graded responses and fitness costs in tadpole antipredator defences. Oikos 107:90–99

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Littlejohn MJ, Harrison PA (1985) The functional significance of the diphasic advertisement call of Geocrinia victoriana (Anura: Leptodactylidae). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 16:363–373

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lopez PT, Narins PM, Lewis ER, Moore SW (1988) Acoustically induced call modification in the white-lipped frog, Leptodactylus albilabris. Anim Behav 36:1295–1308

    Google Scholar 

  • Marshall VT, Humfeld SC, Bee MA (2003) Plasticity of aggressive signaling and its evolution in male spring peepers, Pseudacris crucifer. Anim Behav 65:1223–1234

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martins EP (1994) Structural complexity in a lizard communication system: the Sceloporus graciosus “push-up” display. Copeia 1994:944–955

    Google Scholar 

  • Martins M, Pombal JP, Haddad CFB (1998) Escalated aggressive behaviour and facultative parental care in the nest building gladiator frog, Hyla faber. Amphibia-Reptilia 19:65–73

    Google Scholar 

  • Martof B (1956) Factors influencing size and composition of populations of Rana clamitans. Am Midl Nat 56:224–245

    Google Scholar 

  • Narins PM, Lewis ER, McClelland BE (2000) Hyperextended call note repertoire of the endemic Madagascar treefrog Boophis madagascariensis (Rhacophoridae). J Zool, Lond 250:283–298

    Google Scholar 

  • Owen PC (2003) The structure, function, and evolution of aggressive signals in anuran amphibians. University of Connecticut, Ph.D. Dissertation

  • Owen PC, Perrill SA (1998) Habituation in the green frog, Rana clamitans. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 44:209–213

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Payne RJH, Pagel M (1997) Why do animals repeat displays? Anim Behav 54:109–119

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pengilley RK (1971) Calling and associated behaviour of some species of Pseudophryne (Anura: Leptodactylidae). J Zool, Lond 163:73–92

    Google Scholar 

  • Pusey AE, Packer C (1997) The ecology of relationships. In: Krebs JR, Davies NB (eds) Behavioral ecology, an evolutionary approach, 4th ed. Blackwell Science, Oxford, pp 254–283

    Google Scholar 

  • Quinn GP, Keough MJ (2002) Experimental design and data analysis for biologists. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramer JD, Jenssen TA, Hurst CJ (1983) Size-related variation in the advertisement call of Rana clamitans (Anura: Ranidae), and its effect on conspecific males. Copeia 1983:141–155

    Google Scholar 

  • Robertson JGM (1986) Male territoriality, fighting, and assessment of fighting ability in the Australian frog Uperolia rugosa. Anim Behav 34:763–772

    Google Scholar 

  • Rose GJ, Brenowitz EA (1991) Aggressive thresholds of male Pacific treefrogs for advertisement calls vary with amplitude of neighbors’ calls. Ethology 89:244–252

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz JJ (1989) Graded aggressive calls of the spring peeper, Pseudacris crucifer. Herpetologica 45:172–181

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz JJ (1994) Male advertisement and female choice in frogs: recent findings and new approaches to the study of communication in a dynamic acoustic environment. Am Zool 34:616–624

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz JJ (2001) Call monitoring and interactive playback systems in the study of acoustic interactions among male anurans. In: Ryan MJ (ed) Anuran communication. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, pp 183–204

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz JJ, Wells KD (1984) Interspecific acoustic interactions of the neotropical treefrog Hyla ebraccata. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 14:211–224

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz JJ, Wells KD (1985) Intra- and interspecific vocal behavior of the Neotropical treefrog Hyla microcephala. Copeia 1985:27–38

    Google Scholar 

  • Stewart MM, Bishop PJ (1994) Effects of increased sound level of advertisement calls on calling male frogs, Eleutherodactylus coqui. J Herpetol 28:46–53

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner WE Jr (1989a) Fighting, assessment, and frequency alteration in Blanchard’s cricket frog. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 25:429–436

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner WE Jr (1989b) Graded aggressive signals in Blanchard’s cricket frog: vocal responses to opponent proximity and size. Anim Behav 38:1025–1038

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner WE Jr (1992) Deceptive or honest signalling of fighting ability? A test of alternative hypotheses for the function of changes in dominant frequency by male cricket frogs. Anim Behav 44:449–462

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells KD (1977a) The social behaviour of anuran amphibians. Anim Behav 25:666–693

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells KD (1977b) Territoriality and male mating success in the green frog (Rana clamitans). Ecology 58:750–762

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells KD (1978) Territoriality in the green frog (Rana clamitans): vocalizations and agonistic behaviour. Anim Behav 26:1051–1063

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells KD (1988) The effect of social interactions on anuran vocal behavior. In: Fritzsch B, Ryan MJ, Wilczynski W, Hetherington TE, Walkowiak W (eds) The evolution of the amphibian auditory system. John Wiley and Sons, New York, pp 433–454

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells KD (1989) Vocal communication in a neotropical treefrog, Hyla ebraccata: responses of males to graded aggressive calls. Copeia 1989:461–466

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells KD (2001) The energetics of calling in frogs. In: Ryan MJ (ed) Anuran communication. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, pp 45–60

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells KD, Bard KM (1987) Vocal communication in a neotropical treefrog, Hyla ebraccata: responses of females to advertisement and aggressive calls. Behaviour 101:200–210

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells KD, Schwartz JJ (1984) Vocal communication in a neotropical treefrog, Hyla ebraccata: aggressive calls. Behaviour 91:128–145

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson L, Coward M (2002) Linear models II: analysis of variance. In: Systat 10.2 Statistics I. Systat Software, Richmond, CA, pp 431–486

    Google Scholar 

  • Wogel H, Abrunhosa PA, Pombal JP Jr (2004) Vocalizations and aggressive behavior of Phyllomedusa rohdei (Anura: Hylidae). Herpetol Rev 35:239–243

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the town of Brooklyn, CT, for access to the study site and C. Smith for assistance in the field. We would also like to thank K. Wells, K. Schwenk, E. Adams, M. Bee, J. Christensen-Dalsgaard, and two anonymous reviewers for criticism and comments on this manuscript. L. LaPlante, E. Schultz, and J. Glaz provided additional advice on statistical methods. Financial support for this work was provided by the Ralph M. Wetzel Fund for Vertebrate Biology and a University of Connecticut Graduate School research fellowship to P. Owen. This study complied with the laws of the United States and adhered to the Guidelines for the Treatment of Animals in Behavioral Research and Teaching of the Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour/Animal Behavior Society and the Guidelines for Use of Live Amphibians and Reptiles in Field and Laboratory Research of the American Society of Ichthyologists and Herpetologists

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Patrick C. Owen.

Additional information

Communicated by J. Christensen-Dalsgaard

Appendix

Appendix

Table A Responses for each individual ordered according to stimulus intensity for variables with significant linear changes. Means and standard deviations (SD) are displayed

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Owen, P.C., Gordon, N.M. The effect of perceived intruder proximity and resident body size on the aggressive responses of male green frogs, Rana clamitans (Anura: Ranidae). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 58, 446–455 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-005-0961-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-005-0961-3

Keywords

Navigation