Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Locating food in a spatially heterogeneous environment: Implications for fitness of the macrodecomposer Armadillidium vulgare (Isopoda: Oniscidea)

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

To assess the fitness consequences of foraging on patchy resources, consumption rates, growth rates and survivorship of Armadillidium vulgare were monitored while feeding in arenas in which the spatial distribution of patches of high quality food (powdered dicotyledonous leaf litter) was varied within a matrix of low quality food (powdered grass leaf litter). Predictions from behavioural experiments that these fitness correlates would be lower when high quality food is more heterogeneously distributed in space were tested but not supported either by laboratory or field experiments. To investigate whether A. vulgare can develop the ability to relocate high quality food patches, changes in foraging behaviour, over a comparable time period to that used in the fitness experiments, were monitored in arenas in which there was a high quality food patch in a low quality matrix. A. vulgare increased its ability to relocate the position of high quality food over time. It reduced time spent in low quality food matrices and increased time spent in high quality food patches with time after the start of the experiment. When the position of a high quality food patch was moved, the time spent in the low quality food matrix increased and less time was spent in high quality food patches, compared to arenas in which the food was not moved. The fitness benefits for saprophages of developing the ability to relocate high quality patches while foraging in spatially heterogeneous environments are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Belovsky GE (1978) Diet optimization in a generalist herbivore: moose. Theor Popul Biol 14:105–134

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Belovsky GE (1984) Herbivore optimal foraging: a comparative test of three models. Am Nat 124:97–115

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernays EA, Chapman RF (1994) Host-plant selection by phytophagous insects. Chapman & Hall, New York, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernays EA, Wrubel RP (1985) Learning by grasshoppers: association of colour/light intensity with food. Physiol Entomol 10:359–369

    Google Scholar 

  • Cadish G, Giller KE (1997) Driven by nature: plant litter quality and decomposition. CAB International, Wallingford

    Google Scholar 

  • Charnov EL (1976) Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem. Theor Popul Biol 9:129–136

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crawley MJ (1983) Herbivory: the dynamics of animal–plant interactions. Blackwell Scientific Publications, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham JP, West SA, Wright DJ (1998) Learning in the nectar foraging behaviour of Helicoverpa armigera. Ecol Entomol 23:363–369

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cuthill IC, Kacelnik A, Krebs JR, Haccou P, Icurasa Y (1990) Patch use by starlings: the effect of recent experience on foraging decisions. Anim Behav 40:625–640

    Google Scholar 

  • De Perera TB (2004) Spatial parameters encoded in the spatial map of the blind Mexican cave fish, Astyanax fasciatus. Anim Behav 68:291–295

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dukas R, Real LA (1991) Learning foraging tasks by bees: a comparison between social and solitary species. Anim Behav 42:269–276

    Google Scholar 

  • Dyer FC (1996) Spatial memory and navigation by honeybees on the scale of the foraging range. J Exp Biol 199:147–154

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Egas M, Norde DJ, Sabelis MW (2003) Adaptive learning in arthropods: spider mites learn to distinguish food quality. Exp Appl Acarol 30:233–247

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg LA, Hart WE, Wilson DB (1999) Learning foraging thresholds for lizards: an analysis of a simple learning algorithm. J Theor Biol 197:361–369

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gunnarsson T (1987) Selective feeding on a maple leaf by Oniscus asellus (Isopoda). Pedobiology 30:161–165

    Google Scholar 

  • Hammer M, Menzel R (1995) Learning and memory in the honeybee. J Neurosci 15:1617–1630

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hassall M (1996) Spatial variation in favourability of a grass heath as a habitat for woodlice (Isopoda: Oniscidea). Pedobiology 40:514–528

    Google Scholar 

  • Hassall M, Riddington R, Helden A (2001) Foraging behaviour of brent geese, Branta b. bernicla, on grasslands: effects of sward length and nitrogen content. Oecologia 127:97–104

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hassall M, Rushton SP (1984) Feeding behaviour of terrestrial isopods in relation to plant defences and microbial decay. In: Sutton SL, Holdich D (eds) Proceedings of the Zoological Society of London, Symposium No. 53, pp 487–505

  • Hassall M, Tuck JM, Smith DW, Gilroy JJ, Addison RK (2002) Effects of spatial heterogeneity on feeding behaviour of Porcellio scaber (Isopoda: Oniscidea). Eur J Soil Biol 38:53–57

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmann G (1985) The influence of landmarks on the systematic search behavior of the desert isopod Hemilepistus reaumuri. 1. Role of the landmark made by the animal. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 17:325–334

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howery LD, Bailey DW, Ruyle GB, Renken WJ (2000) Cattle use visual cues to track food locations. Appl Anim Behav Sci 67:1–14

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes RN, Blight CM (2000) Two intertidal fish species use visual association learning to track the status of food patches in a radial maze. Anim Behav 59:613–621

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes RN, Seed R (1995) Behavioural mechanisms of prey selection in crabs. J Exp Marine Biol Ecol 193:225–238

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krebs JR, Inman AJ (1992) Learning and foraging: individuals, groups and populations. Am Nat 140 Suppl 5:563–584

    Google Scholar 

  • Krebs JR, Kacelnik A, Taylor P (1978) Test of optimal sampling by foraging great tits. Nature 275:27–31

    Google Scholar 

  • Krebs JR, McCleery RH (1984) Optimization in behavioural ecology. In: Krebs JR, Davies NB (eds) Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach. Blackwell Scientific Publications, Oxford, pp 91–121

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawlor LR (1976) Parent investment and offspring fitness in the terrestrial isopod Armadillidum vulgare (Latr.) (Crustacea: Oniscoidea). Evolution 31:775–785

    Google Scholar 

  • McNaughton SJ (1988) Mineral nutrition and spatial concentrations of African ungulates. Nature 334:343–345

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McNaughton SJ (1990) Mineral nutrition and seasonal movements of African migratory ungulates. Nature 345:613–615

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ney-Nifle M, Keasar T, Shmida A (2001) Location and colour learning in bumblebees in a two-phase conditioning experiment. J Insect Behav 14:697–711

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parsons AJ, Dumont B (2003) Spational heterogeneity and grazing processes. Anim Res 52:161–179

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rebar CE (1995) Ability of Dipodomys-merriami and Chaetodipus-intermedius to locate resource distributions. J Mammal 76:437–447

    Google Scholar 

  • Rushton SP, Hassall M (1983) The effects of food quality on the life history parameters of the terrestrial isopod Armadillidium vulgare (Latreille). Oecologia 57:257–261

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rushton SP, Hassall M (1987) Effects of food quality on isopod population dynamics. Funct Ecol 1:359–367

    Google Scholar 

  • Siemers BM (2001) Finding prey by associative learning in gleaning bats: experiments with a Natterer’s bat Myotis nattereri. Acta Chiropterol 3:211–215

    Google Scholar 

  • Shettleworth SJ, Krebs JR, Stephens DW, Gibbon J (1988) Tracking a fluctuating environment: a study of sampling. Anim Behav 36:87–105

    Google Scholar 

  • Stephens DW, Krebs JR (1986) Foraging theory. In: Krebs JR, Clutton-Brock T (eds) Monographs in behavior and ecology. Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey

    Google Scholar 

  • Sumpter DJT, Beekman M (2003) From nonlinearity to optimality: pheromone trail foraging by ants. Anim Behav 66:273–280

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Takeda N (1984) The aggregation phenomenon in terrestrial isopods. In: Sutton SL, Holdich DM (eds) The biology of terrestrial isopods. Proceedings of a symposium held at the Zoological Society of London on 7th and 8th of July 1983, vol 53, pp 381–404, Clarendon, Oxford

  • Tamm S (1987) Tracking varying environments:sampling by hummingbirds. Anim Behav 35:1725–1734

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen N, van Kruyt W (1938) Über die Orientierung des Bienenwolfes (Philanthus triangulum Fabr.). III. Die Bevorzugung bestimmter Wegmarken. Z Vergl Physiol 25:292–334

    Google Scholar 

  • Tuck JM (2001) Effects of spatial heterogeneity on the ecology of terrestrial isopods. PhD Thesis, University of East Anglia, Norwich

  • Tuck JM, Hassall M (2004) Foraging behaviour of Armadillidium vulgare (Isopoda: Oniscidea) in heterogeneous environments. Behaviour 141:233–244

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tumlinson JH, Turlings TCJ, Lewis WJ (1993) Semiochemically mediated foraging behavior in beneficial parasitic insects. Arch Insect Biochem Physiol 22:385–391

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Vet LEM, De Jong AG, Franchi E, Papaj DR (1998) The effect of complete versus incomplete information on odour discrimination in a parasitic wasp. Anim Behav 55:1271–1279

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Warburton K (2003) Learning of foraging skills by fish. Fish Fisheries 4:203–215

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White TCR (1993) The inadequate environment: nitrogen and the abundance of animals. Springer-Verlag, Berlin

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilmshurst JF, Fryxell JM, Hudson RJ (1995) Forage quality and patch choice by wapiti (Cervus elaphus). Behav Ecol 6:209–217

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmer M, Kautz G, Topp W (1996) Olfaction in terrestrial isopods (Isopoda: Oniscidea): the responses of Porcellio scaber to the odour of litter. Eur J Soil Biol 32:141–147

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are very grateful to Jenny Stevenson for help with the fieldwork. This work was funded by a studentship from the Natural Environment Research Council. We thank two anonymous referees for helpful comments on an earlier version of this paper

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Joanne M. Tuck.

Additional information

Communicated by D. Gwynne

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Tuck, J.M., Hassall, M. Locating food in a spatially heterogeneous environment: Implications for fitness of the macrodecomposer Armadillidium vulgare (Isopoda: Oniscidea). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 58, 545–551 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-005-0959-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-005-0959-x

Keywords

Navigation