Advertisement

Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

, Volume 58, Issue 6, pp 566–577 | Cite as

Seasonal variation in feeding behavior, competition and female social relationships in a forest dwelling guenon, the blue monkey (Cercopithecus mitis stuhlmanni), in the Kakamega Forest, Kenya

  • Karen PazolEmail author
  • Marina Cords
Original Article

Abstract

Socioecological models relate differences in feeding strategies to variation in the nature of female social relationships. Among the African forest guenons, females consume large quantities of fruit and other plant reproductive parts, resources which are thought to promote contest competition, yet these monkeys have been characterized as having agonistically undifferentiated relationships in which rank, if discernible at all, does not correlate with fitness benefits. To determine whether female relationships become more hierarchical under relevant ecological conditions, we monitored the adult females of two blue monkey groups (Cercopithecus mitis stuhlmanni) over a complete annual cycle in the Kakamega Forest, Kenya. Females competed aggressively for plant reproductive parts more often than any other resource type, and in both groups we detected linear dominance hierarchies. Nonetheless, agonism rates remained low throughout our study, and did not vary with changes in ecological conditions. Rather, when plant reproductive parts were scarce, subordinate females spent more time feeding and less time resting in an apparent attempt to compensate for a reduced efficiency of food intake. The effects of rank and food abundance were not reflected, however, in the distribution of grooming. The use of alternative feeding strategies appeared to blunt competition – females of all ranks were unlikely to be near others while feeding and spent more time consuming alternative resources when plant reproductive parts were scarce. The diverse diet of this species may allow females to avoid conflict so that dominance has only subtle effects that are difficult to detect. While socioecological models often simplify the connection between resources and female interactions, our results emphasize that the behavior of animals consuming particular resources, and not the resources themselves, are critical predictors of social patterns.

Keywords

Dominance rank Feeding competition Resource utilization Socioecology 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We thank the Government of Kenya for permission to conduct research in the Kakamega Forest and the Institute for Primate Research (National Museums of Kenya) for local sponsorship. We are most grateful to Praxides Akelo and Benjamin Okalo for assistance observing monkeys in the field, to Benjamin Okalo and Wilberforce Okeka for help with identifying the local trees, and to the research teams of 1997 and 1998 for their observations of rare events. Dorothy Cheney, Julie Gros-Louis, Robert Harding and four anonymous reviewers provided helpful suggestions on earlier versions of this manuscript. This research was funded by the Anthropology Department of the University of Pennsylvania, the Wenner Gren Foundation, the L. S. B. Leakey Foundation, and two National Science Foundation grants to M.C. (SBR 95-23623, BCS 98-08273). The investigations described herein comply with the laws of the United States and Kenya.

References

  1. Alexander RD (1974) The evolution of social behavior in groups. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 5:325–383CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Altmann J, Muruthi P (1988) Differences in daily life between semiprovisioned and wild-feeding baboons. Am J Primatol 15:213–221Google Scholar
  3. Barrett L, Henzie SP, Weingrill T, Lycett JE, Hill RA (1999) Market forces predict grooming reciprocity in female baboons. Proc Roy Soc Biol Sci Ser B 266:665–670CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Barton RA (1993) Sociospatial mechanisms of feeding competition among female olive baboons, Papio anubis. Anim Behav 46:791–802CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Barton RA, Byrne RW, Whiten A (1996) Ecology, feeding competition and social structure in baboons. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38:321–329CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Barton RA, Whiten A (1993) Feeding competition among female olive baboons, Papio anubis. Anim Behav 46:777–789CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bertram BCR (1980) Vigilance and group size in ostriches. Anim Behav 28:278–86Google Scholar
  8. Boccia ML, Laudenslager M, Reite M (1988) Food distribution, dominance, and aggressive behaviors in bonnet macaques. Am J Primatol 16:123–130Google Scholar
  9. Boinski S, Sughrue K, Selvaggi L, Quatrone R, Henry M, Cropp S (2002) An expanded test of the ecological model of primate social evolution: Competitive regimes and female bonding in three species of squirrel monkeys (Saimiri oerstedii, S. boliviensis and S. sciureus). Behaviour 139:227–261CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Boydston EE, Kapheim KM, Szykman M, Holekamp KE (2003) Individual variation in space use by female spotted hyenas. J Mammal 84:1006–1018CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Cavallini P, Nel JAJ (1995) Comparative behaviour and ecology of two sympatric mongoose species (Cynictis penicillata and Galerella pulverulenta). S Afr J Zool 30:46–49Google Scholar
  12. Chapman CA, Chapman LJ, Cords M, Gathua JM, Gautier-Hion A, Lambert JE, Rode K, Tutin CEG, White LJT (2002) Variation in the diets of Cercopithecus species: Differences within forests, among forests, and across species. In: Glenn ME, Cords M (eds) The guenons: Diversity and adaptation in African monkeys. Kluwer Academic Publishers, New York, pp 325–350Google Scholar
  13. Cheney DL (1992) Intragroup cohesion and intergroup hostility: The relations between grooming distributions and intergroup competition among female primates. Behav Ecol 3:334–345Google Scholar
  14. Clutton-Brock TH (1988) Reproductive success in male and female red deer. In: Clutton-Brock TH (ed) Reproductive success: Studies of individual variation in contrasting breeding systems. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 325–343Google Scholar
  15. Clutton-Brock TH, Harvey PH (1977) Primate ecology and social organisation. J Zool, London 183:1–39Google Scholar
  16. Cordeiro NJ (1992) Behaviour of blue monkeys (Cercopithecus mitis) in the presence of crowned eagles (Stephanoaetus coronatus). Folia Primatol 59:203–207Google Scholar
  17. Cords M (1986) Interspecific and intraspecific variation in diet of two forest guenons, Cercopithecus ascanius and C. mitis. J Anim Ecol 55:811–827Google Scholar
  18. Cords M (1987) Mixed-species association of Cercopithecus monkeys in the Kakamega Forest, Kenya. University of California, Publications in Zoology 117:1–109Google Scholar
  19. Cords M (2000) Agonistic and affiliative relationships in a blue monkey group. In: Whitehead P, Jolly C (eds) Old world monkeys. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 453–479Google Scholar
  20. Cords M (2002) Friendship among adult female blue monkeys (Cercopithecus mitis). Behaviour 139:291–314CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Dasilva GL (1994) Diet of Colobus polykomos in Tiwai Island: Selection of food in relation to its seasonal abundance and nutritional quality. Int J Primatol 5:655–665Google Scholar
  22. de Vries H (1995) An improved test of linearity in dominance hierarchies containing unknown or tied relationships. Anim Behav 50:1375–1389CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. de Vries H (1998) Finding a dominance order most consistent with a linear hierarchy: A new procedure and review. Anim Behav 55:827–843CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Dunbar R, Sharman M (1984) Is social grooming altruistic?. Z Tierpsychol 64:163–173Google Scholar
  25. Dunbar RIM (1988) Primate social systems. Croom Helm, LondonGoogle Scholar
  26. Dunbar RIM (1992) Time: A hidden constraint on the behavioural ecology of baboons. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 31:35–49CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Fashing PJ, Cords M (2000) Diurnal primate densities and biomass in the Kakamega Forest: An evaluation of census methods and a comparison with other forests. Am J Primatol 50:139–152CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Gautier-Hion A (1988) The diet and dietary habits of forest guenons. In: Gautier-Hion A, Bourliere F, Gautier J, Kingdon J (eds) A primate radiation: Evolutionary biology of the African guenons. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 257–283Google Scholar
  29. Gautier-Hion A, Tutin CEG (1988) Simultaneous attack by adult males of a polyspecific troop of monkeys against a crowned hawk eagle. Folia Primatol 51:149–151PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Gilchrist JS, Otali E (2002) The effects of refuse-feeding on home-range use, group size, and intergroup encounters in the banded mongoose. Can J Zool 80:1795–1802CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Gore MA (1993) Effects of food distribution on foraging competition in rhesus monkeys, Macaca mulatta, and hamadryas baboons, Papio hamadryas. Anim Behav 45:773–786CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Hall CL, Fedigan LM (1997) Spatial benefits afforded by high rank in white-faced capuchins. Anim Behav 53:1069–1082CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Hemelrijk CK (1990) Models of and tests for reciprocity unidirectionality and other social interaction patterns at a group level. Anim Behav 39:1013–1029Google Scholar
  34. Henzi SP, Barrett L (1999) The value of grooming to female primates. Primates 40:47–59Google Scholar
  35. Holekamp KE, Smale L, Szykman M (1996) Rank and reproduction in the female spotted hyaena. J Reprod Fertil 108:229–37PubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Isbell LA (1991) Contest and scramble competition: Patterns of female aggression and ranging behavior among primates. Behav Ecol 2:143–155Google Scholar
  37. Isbell LA, Enstam KL (2002) Predator (in)sensitive foraging in sympatric female vervets (Cercopithecus aethiops) and patas monkeys (Erythrocebus patas): A test of ecological models of group dispersion. In: Miller LE (ed) Eat or be eaten: Predator sensitive foraging among primates. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 154–168Google Scholar
  38. Isbell LA, Pruetz JD (1998) Differences between vervets (Cercopithecus aethiops) and patas monkeys (Erythrocebus patas) in agonistic interactions between adult females. Int J Primatol 19:837–855CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Isbell LA, Pruetz JD, Lewis M, Young T (1999) Rank differences in ecological behavior: A comparative study of patas monkeys (Erythrocebus patas) and vervets (Cercopithecus aethiops). Int J Primatol 20:257–272CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Isbell LA, Pruetz JD, Young TP (1998) Movements of vervets (Cercopithecus aethiops) and patas monkeys (Erythrocebus patas) as estimators of food resource size, density, and distribution. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 42:123–133CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Isbell LA, Young TP (2002) Ecological models of female social relationships in primates: Similarities, disparities, and some directions for future clarity. Behaviour 139:177–202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Janson CH (1988) Intra-specific food competition and primate social structure: A synthesis. Behaviour 105:1–17Google Scholar
  43. Janson CH (1990) Ecological consequences of individual spatial choice in foraging groups of brown capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella. Anim Behav 40:922–934Google Scholar
  44. Janson CH (1992) Evolutionary ecology of primate social structure. In: Smith EA, Winterhalder B (eds) Evolutionary ecology and human behavior. de Gruyter, New York, pp 95–130Google Scholar
  45. Janson CH (2000) Primate socio-ecology: The end of a golden age. Evol Anthropol 9:73–86 DOI: 10.1002/(SICI)1520-6505(2000)9:2<73:AID-EVAN2>3.0.CO;2-XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Koenig A (2000) Competitive regimes in forest-dwelling Hanuman langur females (Semnopithecus entellus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 48:93–109CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Koenig A (2002) Competition for resources and its behavioral consequences among female primates. Int J Primatol 23:759–783CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Koenig A, Beise J, Chalise MK, Ganzhorn JU (1998) When females should contest for food-testing hypotheses about resource density, distribution, size, and quality with Hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 42:225–237CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Korstjens AH, Sterck EHM, Noë R (2002) How adaptive or phylogenetically inert is primate social behaviour? A test with two sympatric colobines. Behaviour 139:203–225CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Lambert JE (1998) Primate digestion: Interactions among anatomy, physiology, and feeding ecology. Evol Anthropol 7:8–20 DOI: 10.1002/(SICI)1520-6505(1998)7:1<8:AID-EVAN3>3.0.CO;2-CCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Lambert JE (2002) Resource switching and species coexistence in guenons: A community analysis of dietary flexibility. In: Glenn ME, Cords M (eds) The guenons: Diversity and adaptation in African monkeys. Kluwer Academic Publishers, New York, pp 309–323Google Scholar
  52. Lawes MJ (1991) Diet of samango monkeys (Cercopithecus mitis erythrarchus) in the Cape Vidal Dune Forest South Africa. J Zool 224:149–173Google Scholar
  53. Magliocca F, Querouil S, Gautier-Hion A (2002) Grouping patterns, reproduction, and dispersal in a population of sitatungas (Tragelaphus spekei gratus). Can J Zool 80:245–250CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Mathy JW, Isbell LA (2001) The relative importance of size of food and interfood distance in eliciting aggression in captive rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Folia Primatol 72:268–277 DOI:10.1159/000049948CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Mitchell CL, Boinski S, van Schaik CP (1991) Competitive regimes and female bonding in two species of squirrel monkeys (Saimiri oerstedi and S. sciureus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 28:55–60CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Nakagawa N (1989) Feeding strategies of Japanese monkeys against deterioration of habitat quality. Primates 30:1–16Google Scholar
  57. Palomares F, Delibes M (1993) Social organization in the Egyptian mongoose: Group size, spatial behaviour and inter-individual contacts in adults. Anim Behav 45:917–925CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Payne HFP, Lawes MJ, Henzi SP (2003) Competition and the exchange of grooming among female samango monkeys (Cercopithecus mitis erythrarchus). Behaviour 140:453–472CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Post DG, Hausfater G, McCuskey SA (1980) Feeding behavior of yellow baboons (Papio cynocephalus): Relationship to age, gender and dominance rank. Folia Primatol 3:179–195Google Scholar
  60. Pruetz JD, Isbell LA (2000) Correlations of food distribution and patch size with agonistic interactions in female vervets (Chlorocebus aethiops) and patas monkeys (Erythrocebus patas) living in simple habitats. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 49:38–47CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Pulliam HR, Caraco T (1984) Living in groups is there an optimal group size?. In: Krebs JR, Davies NB (eds) Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach, 2nd edn. Sunderland, MA, pp 122–147Google Scholar
  62. Robinson JG (1981) Spatial structure in foraging groups of wedge-capped capuchin monkeys Cebus nigrivittatus. Anim Behav 29:1036–1056Google Scholar
  63. Ron T, Henzi SP, Motro U (1996) Do female chacma baboons compete for a safe spatial position in a southern woodland habitat?. Behaviour 133:475–490Google Scholar
  64. Rowell TE, Wilson C, Cords M (1991) Reciprocity and partner preference in grooming of female blue monkeys. Int J Primatol 12:319–336Google Scholar
  65. Rudran R (1978) Socioecology of the blue monkeys (Cercopithecus mitis stuhlmanni) of the Kibale Forest, Uganda. Smithsonian Contrib Zool 249:1–88Google Scholar
  66. Saito C (1996) Dominance and feeding success in female Japanese macaques, Macaca fuscata: Effects of food patch size and inter-patch distance. Anim Behav 51:967–980CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Sterck EHM, Steenbeek R (1997) Female dominance relationships and food competition in the sympatric Thomas langur and long-tailed macaque. Behaviour 134:749–774Google Scholar
  68. Sterck EHM, Watts DP, van Schaik CP (1997) The evolution of female social relationships in nonhuman primates. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 41:291–309CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Strier KB (1994) Myth of the typical primate. Yearbook Phys Anthropol 37:233–277Google Scholar
  70. Struhsaker TT (1978) Food habits of five monkey species in the Kibale Forest, Uganda. In: Chivers DJ, Herbert J (eds) Recent advances in primatology, vol 1. Academic Press, London, pp 225–248Google Scholar
  71. Struhsaker TT (1997) Ecology of an African rain forest. University Press of Florida, GainesvilleGoogle Scholar
  72. Sugiyama Y, Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with special reference to the effect of artificial feeding. Folia Primatol 39:238–263PubMedGoogle Scholar
  73. van Hooff JARAM, van Schaik CP (1992) Cooperation in competition: The ecology of primate bonds. In: Harcourt AH, de Waal FBM (eds) Coalitions and alliances in humans and other animals. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 357–389Google Scholar
  74. van Noordwijk MA, van Schaik CP (1987) Competition among female long-tailed macaques, Macaca fascicularis. Anim Behav 35:577–589Google Scholar
  75. van Schaik CP (1989) The ecology of social relationships amongst female primates. In: Standen V, Foley RA (eds) Comparative socioecology: The behavioural ecology of humans and other mammals. Blackwell Scientific Publications, Oxford, pp 195–218Google Scholar
  76. van Schaik CP, Kappeler PM (1997) Infanticide risk and the evolution of male–female association in primates. Proc Roy Soc London, B 264:1687–1694Google Scholar
  77. van Schaik CP, van Noordwijk MA (1988) Scramble and contest in feeding competition among female long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Behaviour 105:77–98Google Scholar
  78. White F (1983) The vegetation of Africa. UNESCO, ParisGoogle Scholar
  79. Whitten PL (1983) Diet and dominance among female vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). Am J Primatol 5:139–159Google Scholar
  80. Wrangham RW (1980) An ecological model of female-bonded primate groups. Behaviour 75:262–300Google Scholar
  81. Zuberbuhler K (2002) Effects of natural and sexual selection on the evolution of guenon loud calls. In: Glenn ME, Cords M (eds) The guenons: Diversity and adaptation in African monkeys. Kluwer Academic Publishers, New York, pp 289–308Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2005

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Yerkes National Primate Research CenterEmory UniversityAtlantaUSA
  2. 2.Department of Ecology, Evolution and Environmental BiologyColumbia University 10th floorNew YorkUSA

Personalised recommendations