Advertisement

Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

, Volume 57, Issue 1, pp 50–61 | Cite as

Patterns of participation and free riding in territorial conflicts among ringtailed lemurs (Lemur catta)

  • Charles L. NunnEmail author
  • Robert O. Deaner
Original Article

Abstract

Cooperation in animal social groups may be limited by the threat of “free riding,” the potential for individuals to reap the benefits of other individuals’ actions without paying their share of the costs. Here we investigate the factors that influence individual contributions to group-level benefits by studying individual participation in territorial defense among female ringtailed lemurs (Lemur catta). To control for potentially confounding factors, particularly group size, we studied two semi-free-ranging groups at the Duke University Primate Center. First, we used a combination of experimental and observational methods to investigate the costs and benefits of territorial defense for individual lemurs. We found three indications of costs: physical contact occurred during inter-group encounters, participation in territorial defense was negatively correlated with ambient temperature, and rates of self-directed behaviors increased during encounters. Benefits were more difficult to quantify, but observational and experimental tests suggested that individuals shared the gains of territorial defense by foraging in defended territories. Thus, during experiments in which one of the groups was prevented from defending its territory, the free-ranging group made more frequent incursions into the other group’s territory. Second, we examined variation in participation in territorial defense. Individuals varied significantly in their rates of aggression and genital marking during inter-group encounters. The extensive variation documented among individuals was partially accounted for by dominance rank, kinship and patterns of parental care. However, we found no evidence to suggest that participation was enforced through punishment (policing) or exchange of benefits involving grooming. In conclusion, this study provides further insights into cooperative behavior in mammalian social groups by revealing how the costs and benefits of territoriality influence patterns of individual participation in the context of shared (collective) goods.

Keywords

Lemur catta Territoriality Individual participation Cooperation 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We thank the DUPC staff and our summer research assistants for their support. We also acknowledge helpful discussions or comments from R. Barton, T. Clutton-Brock, C. Gilbert, P. Kappeler, R. Lewis, S. Patek, M. Pereira, C. van Schaik and anonymous reviewers. Support was provided by an NSF graduate student fellowship, Conservation International Primate Action Fund, Duke Arts and Sciences Research Council, and Sigma Xi. This paper is Duke Primate Center publication no. 752. The research presented here was described in Animal Research Protocol no. A124-98-3 and A144-99-3R1, approved on 26 March 1998 and 26 April 1999 by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Duke University.

References

  1. Axelrod R, Hamilton WD (1981) The evolution of cooperation. Science 211:1390–1396PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1988) The evolution of reciprocity in sizable groups. J Theor Biol 132:337–356PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Cavigelli SA, Dubovick T, Levash W, Jolly A, Pitts A (2003) Female dominance status and fecal corticoids in a cooperative breeder with low reproductive skew: ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta). Horm Behav 43:166–179CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Cheney DL (1987) Interactions and relationships between groups. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT (eds) Primate societies. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 267–281Google Scholar
  5. Clutton-Brock T (2002) Breeding together: kin selection and mutualism in cooperative vertebrates. Science 296:69–72CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Clutton-Brock TH, Parker GA (1995) Punishment in animal societies. Nature 373:209–215CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Clutton-Brock TH, O’Riain MJ, Brotherton PNM, Gaynor D, Kansky R, Griffin AS, Manser M (1999) Selfish sentinels in cooperative mammals. Science 284:1640–1644CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Clutton-Brock TH, Brotherton PNM, O’Riain MJ, Griffin AS, Gaynor D, Sharpe L, Kansky R, Manser MB, McIlrath GM (2000) Individual contributions to babysitting in a cooperative mongoose, Suricata suricatta. Proc R Soc Lond B 267:301–305CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Connor RC (1995) Altruism among non-relatives: alternatives to the ‘prisoner’s dilemma’. Trends Ecol Evol 10:84–86CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Crowley PH, Provencher L, Sloane S, Dugatkin LA, Spohn B, Rogers L, Alfieri M (1996) Evolving cooperation: the role of individual recognition. BioSystems 37:49–66CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Dugatkin LA (1990) N-person games and the evolution of co-operation: a model based on predator inspection in fish. J Theor Biol 142:123–135PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Dugatkin LA (1997) The evolution of cooperation. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  13. Frank SA (1995) Mutual policing and repression of competition in the evolution of cooperative groups. Nature 377:520–522CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Frank SA (1996) Policing and group cohesion when resources vary. Anim Behav 52:1163–1169CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Hardin G (1968) The tragedy of the commons. Science 162:1243–1248Google Scholar
  16. Hardin R (1982) Collective action. Johns Hopkins University Press, BaltimoreGoogle Scholar
  17. Heinsohn R, Packer C (1995) Complex cooperative strategies in group-territorial lions. Science 269:1260–1262PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Henry JP (1982) The relation of social to biological processes in disease. Soc Sci Med 16:369–380CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Holst D von (1985) Coping behaviour and stress physiology in male tree shrews (Tupaia belangeri). Fortschr Zool 31:461–469Google Scholar
  20. Hood LC, Jolly A (1995) Troop fission in female Lemur catta at Berenty Reserve, Madagascar. Int J Primatol 16:997–1015Google Scholar
  21. Jolly A (1966) Lemur behavior. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  22. Jolly A, Pride E (1999) Troop histories and range inertia of Lemur catta at Berenty, Madagascar: a 33-year perspective. Int J Primatol 20:359–373CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Jolly A, Rasamimanana HR, Kinnaird MF, O’Brien TG, Crowley H, Harcourt CS, Gardner S, Davidson JM (1993) Territoriality in Lemur catta groups during the birth season at Berenty, Madagascar. In: Kappeler PM, Ganzhorn JU (eds) Lemur social systems and their ecological basis. Plenum, New York, pp 85–109Google Scholar
  24. Jones KC (1983) Inter-troop transfer of Lemur catta males at Berenty, Madagascar. Folia Primatol 40:145–160PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Kappeler PM (1993) Variation in social structure: the effects of sex and kinship on social interactions in three lemur species. Ethology 93:125–145Google Scholar
  26. Kappeler PM (1998) To whom it may concern: the transmission and function of chemical signals in Lemur catta. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 42:411–421CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Lazaro-Perea C, (2001) Intergroup interactions in wild common marmosets, Callithrix jacchus: territorial defence and assessment of neighbours. Anim Behav 62:11–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Lloyd S (1995) Environmental influences on host immunity. In: Grenfell BT, Dobson AP (eds) Ecology of infectious diseases in natural populations. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 327–361Google Scholar
  29. Maestripieri D, Schino G, Aureli F, Troisi A (1992) A modest proposal: displacement activities as an indicator of emotions in primates. Anim Behav 44:967–979Google Scholar
  30. Milinski M, Semmann D, Krambeck HJ (2002) Reputation helps solve the ‘tragedy of the commons’. Nature 415:424–426CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Nakamichi M, Koyama N (1997) Social relationships among ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta) in two free-ranging troops at Berenty Reserve, Madagascar. Int J Primatol 18:73–93CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Nowak M, Sigmund K (1993) A strategy of win-stay, lose-shift that outperforms tit-for-tat in the prisoner’s dilemma game. Nature 364:56–58CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Nowak MA, Sigmund K (1998) Evolution of indirect reciprocity by image scoring. Nature 393:573–577CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Nowak MA, Page KM, Sigmund K (2000) Fairness versus reason in the ultimatum game. Science 289:1773–1775CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Nunn CL (2000) Collective action, “free-riders,” and male extragroup conflict. In: Kappeler P (ed) Primate males. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 192–204Google Scholar
  36. Nunn CL, Lewis RJ (2001) Cooperation and collective action in animal behaviour. In: Noë R, van Hooff JARAM, Hammerstein P (eds) Economics in nature. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 42–66Google Scholar
  37. Nunn CL, Pereira ME (2000) Group histories and offspring sex ratios in ringtailed lemurs (Lemur catta). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 48:18–28CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Olson M (1965) The logic of collective action. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  39. Ostrom E (1990) Governing the commons: the evolution of institutions for collective action. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  40. Ostrom E (2001) Social dilemmas and human behvaior. In: Noë R, Hammerstein P, van Hooff JARM (eds) Economics in nature. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  41. Pereira ME (1993) Agonistic interaction, dominance relations, and ontogenetic trajectories in ringtailed lemurs. In: Pereira ME, Fairbanks LA (eds) Juvenile primates: life history, development, and behavior. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 285–305Google Scholar
  42. Pereira ME, Izard MK (1989) Lactation and care for unrelated infants in forest-living ringtailed lemurs. Am J Primatol 18:101–108Google Scholar
  43. Pereira ME, Kappeler PM (1997) Divergent systems of agonistic relationship in lemurid primates. Behaviour 134:225–274Google Scholar
  44. Rice WR, Gaines SD (1994) Heads I win, tails you lose: testing directional alternative hypotheses in ecological and evolutionary research. Trends Ecol Evol 9:235–237Google Scholar
  45. Richard AF (1987) Malagasy prosimians: female dominance. In: Cheney DL, Smuts BB, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT (eds) Primate societies. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 25–33Google Scholar
  46. Roberts G, Sherratt TN (1998) Development of cooperative relationships through increasing investment. Nature 394:175–179CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Roeder JJ, Fornasieri I, Anderson JR (1994) Yawning in ringtailed lemurs (Lemur catta). In: Roeder JJ, Thierr B, Anderson JR, Herrenschmidt N (eds) Current primatology. Université Louis Pasteur, Strasbourg, pp 77–80Google Scholar
  48. Sachs JL, Mueller UG, Wilcox TP, Bull JJ (2004) The evolution of cooperation. Q Rev Biol 79:135–160CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Sandler T (1992) Collective action. University of Michigan Press, Ann Arbor, Mich.Google Scholar
  50. Sauther ML, Sussman RW (1993) Resource competition in wild populations of ringtailed lemurs (Lemur catta): implications for female dominance. In: Kappeler P, Ganzhorn J (eds) Lemur social systems and their ecological basis. Plenum, New York, pp 135–152Google Scholar
  51. Schaik CP van (1996) Social evolution in primates: the role of ecological factors and male behavior. Proc Br Acad 88:9–31Google Scholar
  52. Semmann D, Krambeck HJ, Milinski M (2004) Strategic investment in reputation. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 56:248–252CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Spong G, Creel S (2004) Effects of kinship on territorial conflicts among groups of lions, Panthera leo. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 55:325–331CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Taylor M, Ward H (1982) Chickens, whales, and lumpy goods: alternative models of public goods provision. Polit Stud 30:350–370Google Scholar
  55. Troisi A (2002) Displacement activities as a behavioral measure of stress in nonhuman primates and human subjects. Stress 5:47–54CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Vick LG, Pereira ME (1989) Episodic targeting aggression and the histories of lemur social groups. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 25:3–12Google Scholar
  57. Watts DP, Mitani JC (2001) Boundary patrols and intergroup encounters in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour 138:299–327CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Wilson ML, Hauser MD, Wrangham RW (2001) Does participation in intergroup conflict depend on numerical assessment, range location, or rank for wild chimpanzees? Anim Behav 61:1203–1216CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Wright J, Berg E, De Kort SR, Khazin V, Maklakov AA (2001) Cooperative sentinel behaviour in the Arabian babbler. Anim Behav 62:973–979CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Zahavi A (1977) Reliability in communication systems and the evolution of altruism. In: Stonehouse B, Perrins C (eds) Evolutionary ecology. University Park Press, Baltimore, pp 253–259Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2004

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Section of Evolution and EcologyUniversity of CaliforniaDavisUSA
  2. 2.Department of NeurobiologyDuke University Medical CenterDurhamUSA
  3. 3.Department of Integrative BiologyUniversity of CaliforniaBerkeleyUSA

Personalised recommendations