Skip to main content
Log in

Socioeconomic status and self–other processing: socioeconomic status predicts interference in the automatic imitation task

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

High-status individuals have been found to be less attuned to the behaviour of others in the social environment, at least in the absence of any specific instructions to pay attention to them. Previous work using neural measures has shown that socioeconomic status (SES) influences the degree to which people are attuned to the actions of others. In particular, individuals from low-SES backgrounds were found to exhibit more mu-suppression, which has been suggested to reflect greater levels of sensorimotor resonance, compared to their high-SES counterparts. However, it is unclear whether such effects on brain activity translate into behaviour. Here, we examined differences in automatic imitation between high-SES and low-SES individuals. The automatic imitation task involves participants making actions in response to a numerical cue, while simultaneously being exposed to an action that is incongruent or congruent with the cued response. Patterns of interference effect reveal the extent to which the congruence of the observed action affects performance of the cued response. Interference thus indexes self–other processing, where high levels of interference suggest an increased susceptibility to the actions of others. Our results show that individuals from low-SES backgrounds exhibit more interference than individuals from high-SES backgrounds. Regression analyses revealed a negative relationship between SES and the degree of interference. Overall, our findings suggest that differences in SES are linked to differences in self–other processing, which could be relevant for broader behavioural tendencies exhibited by individuals at varying levels of a social structure.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adler NE, Epel ES, Castellazzo G, Ickovics JR (2000) Relationship of subjective and objective social class with psychological functioning: preliminary data in healthy White women. Health Psychol 19:586–592

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Aglioti SM, Cesari P, Romani M, Urgesi C (2008) Action anticipation and motor resonance in elite basketball players. Nat Neurosci 11:1109–1116

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Agnew ZK, Bhakoo KK, Puri BK (2007) The human mirror system: a motor resonance theory of mind-reading. Brain Res Rev 54:286–293

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bach P, Tipper SP (2007) Implicit action encoding influences personal-trait judgments. Cognition 102(2):151–178

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernhardt BC, Singer T (2012) The neural basis of empathy. Annu Rev Neurosci 35:1–23

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brass M, Bekkering H, Prinz W (2001) Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. Acta Physiol (Oxf) 106(1–2):3–22

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Burkart JM (2017) Evolution and consequences of sociality. In: Call J, Burghardt GM, Pepperberg IM, Snowdon CT, Zentall T (eds) APA handbook of comparative psychology: basic concepts, methods, neural substrate, and behavior (vol 1); (chapter xxiii). American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, pp 257–271

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Chapais B (1991) Primates and the origins of aggression, power, and politics among humans. In: Loy JD, Peters CB (eds) Understanding behavior: what primate studies tell us about human behavior; understanding behavior (chapter ix). Oxford University Press, New York, pp 190–228

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapais B (2015) Competence and the evolutionary origins of status and power in humans. Hum Nat 26(2):161–183

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coll M, Bird G, Catmur C, Press C (2015) Cross-modal repetition effects in the mu rhythm indicate tactile mirroring during action observation. Cortex J Devoted Study Nerv Syst Behav 63:121–131

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coll M, Press C, Hobson H, Catmur C, Bird G (2017) Crossmodal classification of mu rhythm activity during action observation and execution suggests specificity to somatosensory features of actions. J Neurosci 37(24):5936–5947

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • de Guzman M, Bird G, Banissy MJ, Catmur C (2016) Self-other control processes in social cognition: from imitation to empathy. Philos Trans R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci 371(1686):20150079

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Destin M, Rheinschmidt-Same M, Richeson JA (2017) Status-based identity: a conceptual approach integrating the social psychological study of socioeconomic status and identity. Perspect Psychol Sci 12(2):270–289

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar RIM, Shultz S (2007) Evolution in the social brain. Science 317(5843):1344–1347

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske ST (1993) Controlling other people: the impact of power on stereotyping. Am Psychol 48:621–628

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske ST (2001) Effects of power on bias: power explains and maintains individual, group, and societal disparities. In: Lee-Chai AY, Bargh JA (eds) The use and abuse of power: multiple perspectives on the causes of corruption; the use and abuse of power (chapter xiv). Psychology Press, New York, pp 181–193

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiske ST, Berdahl J (2007) Social power. In: Kruglanski AW, Higgins ET (eds) Social psychology: handbook of basic principles, (chapter xiii), 2nd edn. Guilford Press, New York, pp 678–692

    Google Scholar 

  • Galang CM, Obhi SS (2019) Social power and frontal alpha asymmetry. Cogn Neurosci 10(1):44–56

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galinsky AD, Gruenfeld DH, Magee JC (2003) Power and action. J Pers Soc Psychol 85:453–466

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gillmeister H, Catmur C, Liepelt R, Brass M, Heyes C (2008) Experience-based priming of body parts: a study of action imitation. Brain Res 1217:157–170

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Guinote A (2007) Power and goal pursuit. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 33(8):1076–1087

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes C (2011) Automatic imitation. Psychol Bull 137(3):463–483

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson HM, Bishop DVM (2016) Mu suppression—a good measure of the human mirror neuron system? Cortex J Devoted Study Nerv Syst Behav 82:290–310

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hogeveen J, Inzlicht M, Obhi SS (2014) Power changes how the brain responds to others. J Exp Psychol Gen 143(2):755–762

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keltner D, Gruenfeld DH, Anderson C (2003) Power, approach, and inhibition. Psychol Rev 110:265–284

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohn ML, Schooler C (1969) Class, occupation, and orientation. Am Sociol Rev 34:659–667

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kraus MW, Côté S, Keltner D (2010) Social class, contextualism, and empathic accuracy. Psychol Sci 21:1716–1723

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kraus MW, Piff PK, Mendoza-Denton R, Rheinschmidt ML, Keltner D (2012) Social class, solipsism, and contextualism: how the rich are different from the poor. Psychol Rev 119(3):546–572

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis RJ (2002) Beyond dominance: the importance of leverage. Q Rev Biol 77:149–164

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Obhi SS, Hogeveen J, Giacomin M, Jordan CH (2014) Automatic imitation is reduced in narcissists. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 40(3):920–928

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pfeifer JH, Iacoboni M, Mazziotta JC, Dapretto M (2008) Mirroring others’ emotions relates to empathy and interpersonal competence in children. Neuroimage 39:2076–2085

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piff PK (2014) Wealth and the inflated self: class, entitlement, and narcissism. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 40:34–43

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross CE, Mirowsky J (2008) Neighborhood socioeconomic status and health: context or composition? City Community 7(2):163–179

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith PK, Trope Y (2006) You focus on the forest when you’re in charge of the trees: power priming and abstract information processing. J Pers Soc Psychol 90:578–596

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spengler S, Brass M, Kühn S, Schütz-Bosbach S (2010) Minimizing motor mimicry by myself: self-focus enhances online action-control mechanisms during motor contagion. Conscious Cognit Int J 19(1):98–106

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Troy AS, Ford BQ, McRae K, Zarolia P, Mauss IB (2017) Change the things you can: emotion regulation is more beneficial for people from lower than from higher socioeconomic status. Emotion 17(1):141–154

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Kleef GA, Oveis C, van der Löwe I, LuoKogan A, Goetz J, Keltner D (2008) Power, distress, and compassion: turning a blind eye to the suffering of others. Psychol Sci 19:1315–1322

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Varnum MEW, Blais C, Brewer GA (2016) Social class affects mu-suppression during action observation. Soc Neurosci 11(4):449–454

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waytz A, Mitchell JP (2011) Two mechanisms for simulating other minds: dissociations between mirroring and self-projection. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 20(3):197–200

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Sumeet Farwaha or Sukhvinder S. Obhi.

Additional information

Communicated by Carlo Alberto Marzi.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Farwaha, S., Obhi, S.S. Socioeconomic status and self–other processing: socioeconomic status predicts interference in the automatic imitation task. Exp Brain Res 238, 833–841 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-020-05761-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-020-05761-7

Keywords

Navigation