Skip to main content
Log in

Body–foot geometries as revealed by perturbed obstacle position with different time constraints

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examined the geometrical relationships between the feet, pelvis and an environmental obstruction when crossing an obstacle with unexpected changes to its position. Nine healthy young adults stepped over an obstacle 19 cm high with their right leg leading. The obstacle could be static or advanced at either lead (early detection) or trail (late detection) foot contact prior to clearance to force an adaptive reorganization of body–foot geometry and foot proximity to the obstacle. Stride length, minimum foot clearance over the obstacle, and foot-obstacle horizontal proximity before and after clearance were measured along with the relative position of the pelvis to each foot at eight points (four for each foot) during approach and clearance: heel contacts before and after crossing the obstacle, maximum foot heights and foot clearances. With early obstacle movement, trail limb stride length before crossing was lengthened, but foot proximity was still far from the final obstacle position. Clearance was less affected for the trail foot as compared to the lead foot. Proximity of the lead limb following clearance was the same for both early and late perturbations and closer than for the static obstacle condition. For relative body–foot positioning, significant differences were found only in the anterior-posterior direction. Following obstacle displacement, body–foot geometry was initially adapted, but then re-established to static obstacle values with an apparent focus on a balance geometry with the forward placed foot establishing new contact. These findings support an overall balance geometry that can be temporarily adjusted and coordinated with foot proximity to the obstruction to maintain continual gait and safe clearance.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Armand M, Huissoon JP, Patla AE (1998) Stepping over obstacles during locomotion: insights from multiobjective optimization on set of input parameters. Trans Rehabil Eng 6:43–52

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Carlsen AN (2015) A broadband acoustic stimulus is more likely than a pure tone to elicit a startle reflex and prepared movements. Physiol Rep 3(8):e12509

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chou L-S, Draganich LF (1997) Stepping over an obstacle increases the motions and moments of the joints of the trailing limb in young adults. J Biomech 30:331–337

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chou L-S, Draganich LF (1998) Placing the trailing foot closer to an obstacle reduces flexion of the hip, knee, and ankle to increase the risk of tripping. J Biomech 31:685–691

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chou LS, Kenton K, Brey R, Draganich L (2001) Motion of the whole body’s center of mass when stepping over obstacles of different heights. Gait Posture 13:17–26

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Da Silva JJ, Barbieri FA, Gobbi LTB (2011) Adaptive locomotion for crossing a moving obstacle. Mot Control 15:419–433

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hak L, Houdijk H, Steenbrink F, Mert A, van der Wurff P, Beek PJ, van Dieen JH (2012) Speeding up or slowing down? Gait adaptations to preserve gait stability in response to balance perturbations. Gait Posture 36:260–264

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hak L, Houdijk H, Steenbrink F, Mert A, van der Wurff P, Beek PJ, van Dieën JH (2013) Stepping strategies for regulating gait adaptability and stability. J Biomech 46:905–911

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heijnen M, Muir B, Rietdyk S (2012) Factors leading to obstacle contact during adaptive locomotion. Exp Brain Res 223:219–231

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hof AL (2008) The “extrapolated center of mass” concept suggests a simple control of balance in walking. Hum Mov Sci 27:112–125

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hof AL, Gazendam MG, Sinke WE (2005) The condition for dynamic stability. J Biomech 38:1–8

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lajoie K, Drew T (2007) Lesions of area 5 of the posterior parietal cortex in the cat produce errors in the accuracy of paw placement during visually guided locomotion. J Neurophysiol 97(3):2339–2354

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lajoie K, Bloomfield LW, Nelson FJ, Suh JJ, Marigold DS (2012) The contribution of vision, proprioception, and efference copy in storing a neural representation for guiding trail leg trajectory over an obstacle. J Neurophysiol 107:2283–2293

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loverro K, Mueske N, Hamel K (2013) Location of minimum foot clearance on the shoe and with respect to the obstacle changes with locomotor task. J Biomech 46:1842–1850

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • MacLellan M (2017) Modular organization of muscle activity patterns in the leading and trailing limbs during obstacle clearance in healthy adults. Exp Brain Res 235:2011–2026

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McFadyen BJ, Winter DA (1991) Anticipatory locomotor adjustments during obstructed human walking. Neurosci Res Commun 9:37–44

    Google Scholar 

  • McFadyen BJ, Magnan GA, Boucher JP (1993) Anticipatory locomotor adjustments for avoiding visible, fixed obstacles of varying proximity. Hum Mov Sci 21:259–272

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McFadyen BJ, Winter DA, Allard F (1994) Simulated control of unilateral, anticipatory locomotor adjustments during obstructed gait. Biol Cybern 72:151–160

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McFadyen BJ, Bouyer L, Bent LR, Inglis JT (2007) Visual-vestibular influences on locomotor adjustments for stepping over an obstacle. Exp Brain Res 179(2):235–243

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mohagheghi AA, Moraes R, Patla AE. (2004) The effects of distant and on-line visual information on the control of approach phase and step over an obstacle during locomotion. Exp Brain Res 155:459–468

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Noguchi K, Gel YR, Brunner E, Konietschke F (2012) nparLD: an R software package for the nonparametric analysis of longitudinal data in factorial experiments. J Stat Softw 50(12):1–23 https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v050.i12

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Novak AC, Deshpande N (2014) Effects of aging on whole body and segmental control while obstacle crossing under impaired sensory conditions. Hum Mov Sci 35:121–130

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Patla AE, Beuter A, Prentice S (1991) A two stage correction of the limb trajectory to avoid obstacles during stepping. Neurosci Res Commun 8:153–159

    Google Scholar 

  • Patla AE, Rietdyk S, Martin C, Prentice S (1996) Locomotor patterns of the leading and the trailing limbs as solid and fragile obstacles are stepped over: some insights into the role of vision during locomotion. J Mot Behav 28:35–47

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Redfern MS, Schumann T (1994) A model of foot placement during gait. J Biomech 27:1339–1346

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rhea CK, Rietdyk S (2007) Visual exteroceptive information provided during obstacle crossing did not modify the lower limb trajectory. Neurosci Lett 418(1):60–65

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roll R, Kavounoudias A, Roll JP (2002) Cutaneous afferents from human plantar sole contribute to body posture awareness. Neuroreport 13(15):1957–1961

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Verrel J, Lövden M, Lindenberger U (2010) Motor-equivalent covariation stabilizes step parameters and center of mass position during treadmill walking. Exp Brain Res 207:13–26

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang Y, Srinivasan M (2014) Stepping in the direction of the fall: the next foot placement can be predicted from current upper body state in steady-state walking. Biol Lett 10:20140405

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Weerdesteyn V, Nienhuis B, Hampsink B, Duysens J (2004) Gait adjustments in response to an obstacle are faster than voluntary reactions. Hum Mov Sci 23:351–363

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This study was financially supported through operating Grants from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research of Canada (BJM, LJB) and by student bursaries to L-PD from Laval University and the Centre for Interdisciplinary Research in Rehabilitation and Social Integration. The combined assistance of Isabelle Lorusso and Frédéric Dumont for analyses, Jean Leblond for statistical consultation, Guy St-Vincent in data collection and Steve Forest for obstacle construction was greatly appreciated.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bradford J. McFadyen.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Dugas, LP., Bouyer, L.J. & McFadyen, B.J. Body–foot geometries as revealed by perturbed obstacle position with different time constraints. Exp Brain Res 236, 711–720 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-017-5161-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-017-5161-7

Keywords

Navigation