Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 236, Issue 2, pp 505–516 | Cite as

Temperature influences perception of the length of a wielded object via effortful touch

  • Madhur Mangalam
  • Jeffrey B. Wagman
  • Karl M. Newell
Research Article

Abstract

Individuals can perceive the properties of an attached or grasped object by wielding it through muscular effort—an ability referred to as dynamic or effortful touch. Sensitivity to the forces required to move such objects and to the resulting global patterns of tissue deformation underlies such perception. Given that perception via dynamic touch is movement-based, we hypothesized that manipulations that affect the ability to produce and control muscular movements might affect perception via dynamic touch. Cooling muscles from 40 to 10 °C impedes the development and transmission of muscular force and diminishes muscle stretch-reflex sensitivity. Accordingly, we anticipated that changes in hand temperature would alter the ability to detect patterns of tissue deformation and thus perception of the properties of wielded objects. In two experiments, participants wielded dowels with different lengths and rotational inertias (Experiment 1) and objects with identical lengths and different rotational inertias (Experiment 2). They reported perceived lengths of these objects, in the absence of vision, in cool (~ 10 °C), neutral (~ 30 °C), and warm temperature conditions (~ 40 °C). Actual length predicted perceived length of the dowels (Experiment 1), and rotational inertia predicted perceived length of the objects (Experiment 2); perceived lengths were longer in the warm condition than in the cool condition. In consideration of known temperature-induced changes in tissue structure and function, our results support the hypothesis that comparable processes underlie the control of movement and perception via dynamic touch.

Keywords

Dynamic touch Effortful touch Haptic perception Invariant Proprioception 

Notes

Author contributions

MM and KMN conceived and designed research; MM performed experiments; MM analyzed data; MM and JBW interpreted results of experiments; MM prepared figures; MM, JBW, and KMN edited and revised manuscript; MM, JBW, and KMN approved final version of manuscript.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

References

  1. Bernstein NA (1967) The co-ordination and regulation of movement. Pergamon, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  2. Bernstein NA, Latash ML, Turvey MT (1996) Dexterity and its development. Lawrence Erlbaum, MarwahGoogle Scholar
  3. Burton G, Turvey MT (1990) Perceiving the lengths of rods that are held but not wielded. Ecol Psychol 2:295–324.  https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326969eco0204_1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Burton G, Turvey MT, Solomon HY (1990) Can shape be perceived by dynamic touch? Percept Psychophys 48:477–487.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03211592 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Carello C, Turvey MT (2000) Rotational invariance and dynamic touch. In: Heller MA (ed) Touch, representation and blindness. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 27–66Google Scholar
  6. Carello C, Fitzpatrick P, Domaniewicz I et al (1992) Effortful touch with minimal movement. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 18:290–302.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.18.1.290 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Carello C, Thuot S, Turvey MT (2000) Aging and the perception of a racket’s sweet spot. Hum Mov Sci 19:1–20.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-9457(99)00044-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Carello C, Kinsella-Shaw J, Amazeen EL, Turvey MT (2006) Peripheral neuropathy and object length perception by effortful (dynamic) touch: a case study. Neurosci Lett 405:159–163.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2006.06.047 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Carello C, Silva PL, Kinsella-Shaw JM, Turvey MT (2008) Muscle-based perception: theory, research and implications for rehabilitation. Braz J Phys Ther 12:339–350.  https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-35552008000500002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Clarke RSJ, Hellon RF, Lind AR (1958) The duration of sustained contractions of the human forearm at different muscle temperatures. J Physiol 143:454–473.  https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1958.sp006071 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  11. Cooper MM, Carello C, Turvey MT (2000) Perceptual independence of whole length, partial length, and hand position in wielding a rod. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 26:74–85.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.26.1.74 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Cornwall MW (1994) Effect of temperature on muscle force and rate of muscle force production in men and women. J Orthop Sport Phys Ther 20:74–80.  https://doi.org/10.2519/jospt.1994.20.2.74 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. de Ruiter CJ, Jones DA, Sargeant AJ, de Haan A (1999) Temperature effect on the rates of isometric force development and relaxation in the fresh and fatigued human adductor pollicis muscle. Exp Physiol 84:1137–1150CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Fitzpatrick P, Carello C, Turvey MT (1994) Eigenvalues of the inertia tensor and exteroception by the “muscular sense”. Neuroscience 60:551–568.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0306-4522(94)90264-X CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Gibson JJ (1966) The senses considered as perceptual systems. Houghton Mifflin, BostonGoogle Scholar
  16. Gibson JJ (1979) The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton Mifflin, BostonGoogle Scholar
  17. Hajnal A, Fonseca S, Harrison S et al (2007a) Comparison of dynamic (effortful) touch by hand and foot. J Mot Behav 39:82–88.  https://doi.org/10.3200/JMBR.39.2.82-88 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Hajnal A, Fonseca S, Kinsella-Shaw JM et al (2007b) Haptic selective attention by foot and by hand. Neurosci Lett 419:5–9.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2007.03.042 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Jarmey C, Sharkey J (2003) The concise book of muscles. North Atlantic Books, BerkeleyGoogle Scholar
  20. Jones LA, Lederman SJ (2006) Human hand function. Oxford University Press, New YorkCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Kenshalo DRS (1986) Somesthetic sensitivity in young and elderly humans. J Gerontol 41:732–742.  https://doi.org/10.1093/geronj/41.6.732 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Mangalam M, Barton SA, Wagman JB et al (2017) Perception of the length of an object through dynamic touch is invariant across changes in the medium. Atten Percept Psychophys 79:2499–2509.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-017-1403-9 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Michalski WJ, Séguin JJ (1975) The effects of muscle cooling and stretch on muscle spindle secondary endings in the cat. J Physiol 253:341–356.  https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1975.sp011193 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  24. Mountcastle VB (2005) The sensory hand. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  25. Ottoson D (1965) The effects of temperature on the isolated muscle spindle. J Physiol 180:636–648.  https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1965.sp007721 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  26. Pagano CC, Cabe PA (2003) Constancy in dynamic touch: length perceived by dynamic touch is invariant over changes in media. Ecol Psychol 15:1–17.  https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326969ECO1501_01 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Pagano CC, Donahue KG (1999) Perceiving the lengths of rods wielded in different media. Percept Psychophys 61:1336–1344.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206184 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Pagano CC, Turvey MT (1992) Eigenvectors of the inertia tensor and perceiving the orientation of a hand-held object by dynamic touch. Percept Psychophys 52:617–624.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03211699 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Pagano CC, Fitzpatrick P, Turvey MT (1993) Tensorial basis to the constancy of perceived object extent over variations of dynamic touch. Percept Psychophys 54:43–54.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206936 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Pagano CC, Kinsella-Shaw JM, Cassidy PE, Turvey MT (1994) Role of the inertia tensor in haptically perceiving where an object is grasped. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 20:276–285.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.20.2.276 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Palatinus Z, Carello C, Turvey MT (2011) Principles of part-whole selective perception by dynamic touch extend to the torso. J Mot Behav 43:87–93.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00222895.2010.538767 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Parsons K (2002) Human thermal environments. CRC, Boca RatonCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Ranatunga KW, Sharpe B, Turnbull B (1987) Contractions of a human skeletal muscle at different temperatures. J Physiol 390:383–395.  https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1987.sp016707 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  34. Schleip R, Findley T, Huijing P (2012) Fascia: the tensional network of the human body. Elsevier, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  35. Shaffer SW, Harrison AL (2007) Aging of the somatosensory system: a translational perspective. Phys Ther 87:193–207CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Solomon HY, Turvey MT, Burton G (1989) Perceiving extents of rods by wielding: haptic diagonalization and decomposition of the inertia tensor. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 15:58–68.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.15.1.58 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Stevens JC, Choo KK (1996) Spatial acuity of the body surface over the life span. Somatosens Mot Res 13:153–166.  https://doi.org/10.3109/08990229609051403 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Turvey MT, Carello C (2011) Obtaining information by dynamic (effortful) touching. Philos Trans R Soc London B Biol Sci 366:3123–3132.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0159 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  39. Turvey MT, Fonseca ST (2014) The medium of haptic perception: a tensegrity hypothesis. J Mot Behav 46:143–187.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00222895.2013.798252 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Turvey MT, Burton G, Pagano CC et al (1992) Role of the inertia tensor in perceiving object orientation by dynamic touch. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 18:714–727.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.18.3.714 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Turvey MT, Burton G, Amazeen EL et al (1998) Perceiving the width and height of a hand-held object by dynamic touch. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 24:35–48.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.24.1.35 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. van der Wal JC, Drukker J (1988) The occurrence of muscle spindles in relation to the architecture of the connective tissues in the lateral cubical region of the rat. In: Hník P, Soukup T, Vejsada R, Zelená J (eds) Mechanoreceptors: development, structure, and function. Springer, Boston, pp 345–346Google Scholar
  43. Waddell ML, Amazeen EL (2017) Evaluating the contributions of muscle activity and joint kinematics to weight perception across multiple joints. Exp Brain Res 235:2437–2448.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-017-4979-3 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Wagman JB, Abney DH (2012) Transfer of recalibration from audition to touch: modality independence as a special case of anatomical independence. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 38:589–602CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Wagman JB, Hajnal A (2014a) Task specificity and anatomical independence in perception of properties by means of a wielded object. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 40:2372–2391.  https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000014 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Wagman JB, Hajnal A (2014b) Getting off on the right (or left) foot: perceiving by means of a rod attached to the preferred or non-preferred foot. Exp Brain Res 232:3591–3599.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-014-4047-1 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Wagman JB, Langley MD, Higuchi T (2017) Turning perception on its head: cephalic perception of whole and partial length of a wielded object. Exp Brain Res 235:153–167.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4778-2 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of GeorgiaAthensUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyIllinois State UniversityNormalUSA
  3. 3.Department of KinesiologyUniversity of GeorgiaAthensUSA

Personalised recommendations