Skip to main content
Log in

Eye–hand coordination during visuomotor adaptation: effects of hemispace and joint coordination

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We previously examined adaptive changes of eye–hand coordination during learning of a visuomotor rotation. Gazes during reaching movements were initially directed to a feedback cursor in early practice, but were gradually shifted toward the target with more practice, indicating an emerging gaze anchoring behavior. This adaptive pattern reflected a functional change of gaze control from exploring the cursor–hand relation to guiding the hand to the task goal. The present study further examined the effects of hemispace and joint coordination associated with target directions on this behavior. Young adults performed center-out reaching movements to four targets with their right hand on a horizontal digitizer, while looking at a rotated visual feedback cursor on a computer monitor. To examine the effect of hemispace related to visual stimuli, two out of the four targets were located in the ipsilateral workspace relative to the hand used, the other two in the contralateral workspace. To examine the effect of hemispace related to manual actions, two among the four targets were related to reaches made in the ipsilateral workspace, the other two to reaches made in the contralateral workspace. Furthermore, to examine the effect of the complexity of joint coordination, two among the four targets were reaches involving a direct path from the start to the target involving elbow movements (simple), whereas the other two targets were reaches involving both shoulder and elbow movements (complex). The results showed that the gaze anchoring behavior gradually emerged during practice for reaches made in all target directions. The speed of this change was affected mainly by the hemispace related to manual actions, whereas the other two effects were minimal. The gaze anchoring occurred faster for the ipsilateral reaches than for the contralateral reaches; gazes prior to the gaze anchoring were also directed less at the cursor vicinity but more at the mid-area between the starting point and the target. These results suggest that ipsilateral reaches result in a better predictability of the cursor–hand relation under the visuomotor rotation, thereby prompting an earlier functional change of gaze control through practice from a reactive to a predictive control.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Amenedo E, Pazo-Alvarez P, Cadaveira F (2007) Vertical asymmetries in pre-attentive detection of changes in motion direction. Int J Psychophysiol 64:184–189

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Battaglia-Mayer A, Caminiti R, Lacquaniti F, Zago M (2003) Multiple levels of representation of reaching in the parieto-frontal network. Cereb Cortex 13:1009–1022

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berens P (2009) CircStat: a MATLAB toolbox for circular statistics. J Stat Softw 31(10):1–21

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bock O (1986) Contribution of retinal versus extraretinal signals towards visual localization in goal-directed movements. Exp Brain Res 64:476–482

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bowman MC, Johansson RS, Flanagan JR (2009) Eye–hand coordination in a sequential target contract task. Exp Brain Res 195:273–283

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buch ER, Young S, Contreras-Vidal JL (2003) Visuomotor adaptation in normal aging. Learn Mem 10:55–63

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Carey DP, Liddle J (2013) Hemifield or hemispace: what accounts for the ipsilateral advantages in visually guided aiming? Exp Brain Res 230:323–331

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carey DP, Otto-de Haart EG (2001) Hemispatial differences in visually guided aiming are neither hemispatial nor visual. Neuropsychologia 39:885–894

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carey DP, Hargreaves EL, Goodale MA (1996) Reaching to ipsilateral or contralateral targets: within-hemisphere visuomotor processing cannot explain hemispatial differences in motor control. Exp Brain Res 112:496–504

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carson RG, Chua R, Elliott D, Goodman D (1990) The contribution of vision to asymmetries in manual aiming. Neuropsychologia 28:1215–1220

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Christman SD, Niebauer CL (1997) The relation between left–right and upper–lower visual field asymmetries. In: Christman SD (ed) Cerebral asymmetries in sensory and perceptual processing, vol 123. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 263–296

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Collins T, Schicke T, Röder B (2008) Action goal selection and motor planning can be dissociated by tool use. Cognition 109:363–371

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Contreras-Vidal JL, Buch ER (2003) Effects of Parkinson’s disease on visuomotor adaptation. Exp Brain Res 150:25–32

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Contreras-Vidal JL, Bo J, Boudreau JP, Clark JE (2005) Development of visuomotor representations for hand movement in young children. Exp Brain Res 162:155–164

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cos I, Bélanger N, Cisek P (2011) The influence of predicted arm biomechanics on decision making. J Neurophysiol 105:3022–3033

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cos I, Medleg F, Cisek P (2012) The modulatory influence of endpoint controllability on decisions between actions. J Neurophysiol 108:1764–1780

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crawford JD, Medendorp WP, Marotta JJ (2004) Spatial transformations for eye–hand coordination. J Neurophysiol 92:10–19

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham HA, Vardi I (1990) A vector-sum process produces curved aiming paths under rotated visual-motor mappings. Biol Cybern 64:117–128

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dounskaia N, Van Gemmert AW, Stelmach GE (2000) Interjoint coordination during handwriting-like movements. Exp Brain Res 135:127–140

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dounskaia NV, Ketcham CJ, Stelmach GE (2002) Influence of biomechanical constraints on horizontal arm movements. Mot Control 6:366–387

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dounskaia N, Wang W, Sainburg RL, Przybyla A (2014) Preferred directions of arm movements are independent of visual perception of spatial directions. Exp Brain Res 232:575–586

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elliott D (1988) The influence of visual target and limb information on manual aiming. Can J Psychol 42:57–68

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fisk JD, Goodale MA (1985) The organization of eye and limb movements during unrestricted reaching to targets in contralateral and ipsilateral visual space. Exp Brain Res 60:159–178

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flanagan JR, Lolley S (2001) The inertial anisotropy of the arm is accurately predicted during movement planning. J Neurosci 21:1361–1369

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fourneret P, Jeannerod M (1998) Limited conscious monitoring of motor performance in normal subjects. Neuropsychologia 36:1133–1140

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Galloway JC, Koshland GF (2002) General coordination of shoulder, elbow and wrist dynamics during multijoint arm movements. Exp Brain Res 142:163–180

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goble J, Zhang Y, Shimansky Y, Sharma S, Dounskaia N (2007) Directional biases reveal utilization of arm’s biomechanical properties for optimization of motor behavior. J Neurophysiol 98:1240–1252

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg ME, Bruce CJ (1990) Primate frontal eye fields. III. Maintenance of a spatially accurate saccade signal. J Neurophysiol 64:489–508

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon J, Ghilardi MF, Cooper SE, Ghez C (1994) Accuracy of planar reaching movements. II. Systematic extent errors resulting from inertial anisotropy. Exp Brain Res 99:112–130

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Granek JA, Pisella L, Stemberger J, Vighetto A, Rossetti Y, Sergio LE (2013) Decoupled visually-guided reaching in optic ataxia: differences in motor control between canonical and non-canonical orientations in space. PLoS One 8:e86138

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Greve D, Grossberg S, Guenther F, Bullock D (1993) Neural representations for sensory-motor control, I: head-centered 3-D target positions from opponent eye commands. Acta Psychol (Amst) 82:115–138

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Henriques DY, Klier EM, Smith MA, Lowy D, Crawford JD (1998) Gaze-centered remapping of remembered visual space in an open-loop pointing task. J Neurosci 18:1583–1594

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heuer H, Hegele M (2008) Adaptation to visuomotor rotations in younger and older adults. Psychol Aging 23:190–202

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heuer H, Hegele M, Rand MK (2013) Age-related variations in the control of electronic tools. In: Schlick C, Frieling E, Wegge J (eds) Age-differentiated work systems. Springer, Heidelberg, pp 369–390

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hogan N (1985) The mechanics of multi-joint posture and movement control. Biol Cybern 52:315–331

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horstmann A, Hoffmann KP (2005) Target selection in eye–hand coordination: do we reach to where we look or do we look to where we reach? Exp Brain Res 167:187–195

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Imamizu H, Shimojo S (1995) The locus of visual-motor learning at the task or manipulator level: implications from intermanual transfer. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 21:719–733

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johansson RS, Westling G, Bäckström A, Flanagan JR (2001) Eye–hand coordination in object manipulation. J Neurosci 21:6917–6932

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kagerer FA, Contreras-Vidal JL, Stelmach GE (1997) Adaptation to gradual as compared with sudden visuo-motor distortions. Exp Brain Res 115:557–561

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kim W, Gabbard C, Buchanan JJ, Ryu YU (2007) Right-handers’ reaching in contralateral hemispace: a kinematic observation. J Mot Behav 39:451–456

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kim W, Buchanan J, Gabbard C (2011) Constraints on arm selection processes when reaching: degrees of freedom and joint amplitudes interact to influence limb selection. J Mot Behav 43:403–411

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knoblich G, Kircher TTJ (2004) Deceiving oneself about being in control: conscious detection of changes in visuomotor coupling. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 30:657–666

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krakauer JW, Pine ZM, Ghilardi MF, Ghez C (2000) Learning of visuomotor transformations for vectorial planning of reaching trajectories. J Neurosci 20:8916–8924

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Le Bigot N, Grosjean M (2012) Effects of handedness on visual sensitivity in perihand space. PLoS One 7:e43150

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Le Bigot N, Grosjean M (2016) Exogenous and endogenous shifts of attention in perihand space. Psychol Res 80:677–684

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levine MW, McAnany JJ (2005) The relative capabilities of the upper and lower visual hemifields. Vis Res 45:2820–2830

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mieschke PE, Elliott D, Helsen WF, Carson RG, Coull JA (2001) Manual asymmetries in the preparation and control of goal-directed movements. Brain Cogn 45:129–140

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mussa-Ivaldi FA, Hogan N, Bizzi E (1985) Neural, mechanical, and geometrical factors subserving arm posture in human. J Neurosci 5:2732–2743

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Müsseler J, Sutter C (2009) Perceiving one’s own movements when using a tool. Conscious Cogn 18:359–365

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neggers SFW, Bekkering H (2000) Ocular gaze is anchored to the target of an ongoing pointing movement. J Neurophysiol 83:639–651

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paillard J (1982) The contribution of peripheral and central vision to visually guided reaching. In: Ingle D, Goodale M, Mansfield R (eds) Analysis of visual behaviour. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 367–385

    Google Scholar 

  • Pfann KD, Corcos DM, Moore CG, Hasan Z (2002) Circle-drawing movements at different speeds: role of inertial anisotropy. J Neurophysiol 88:2399–2407

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pouget A, Ducom JC, Torri J, Bavelier D (2002) Multisensory spatial representations in eye-centered coordinates for reaching. Cognition 83:B1–11

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prablanc C, Echallier JE, Jeannerod M, Komilis E (1979) Optimal response of eye and hand motor systems in pointing at a visual target. I. Spatio-temporal characteristics of eye and hand movements and their relationships when varying the amount of visual information. Biol Cybern 35:113–124

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rand MK (2014) Segment interdependency and gaze anchoring during manual two-segment sequences. Exp Brain Res 232:2753–2765

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rand MK, Rentsch S (2016) Eye–hand coordination during visuomotor adaptation with different rotation angles: effects of terminal visual feedback. PLoS One 11:e0164602

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rand MK, Stelmach GE (2010) Effects of hand termination and accuracy constraint on eye–hand coordination during sequential two-segment movements. Exp Brain Res 207:197–211

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reed CL, Grubb JD, Steele C (2006) Hands up: attentional prioritization of space near the hand. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 32:166–177

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reed CL, Betz R, Garza JP, Roberts RJ Jr (2010) Grab it! Biased attention in functional hand and tool space. Attent Percept Psychophys 72:236–245

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rentsch S, Rand MK (2014) Eye–hand coordination during visuomotor adaptation with different rotation angles. PLoS One 9:e109819

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sailer U, Flanagan JR, Johansson RS (2005) Eye–hand coordination during learning of a novel visuomotor task. J Neurosci 25:8833–8842

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shimansky YP (2007) Role of optimization in simple and learning based adaptation and its biologically plausible mechanisms. In: Williams TO (ed) Biological cybernetics research trends. Nova Science Publishers, Hauppauge, pp 95–164

    Google Scholar 

  • Soechting JF, Flanders M (1992) Moving in three-dimensional space: frames of reference, vectors, and coordinate systems. Annu Rev Neurosci 15:167–191

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sülzenbrück S, Heuer H (2009) Functional independence of explicit and implicit motor adjustments. Conscious Cogn 18:145–159

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sülzenbrück S, Heuer H (2011) Type of visual feedback during practice influences the precision of the acquired internal model of a complex visuo-motor transformation. Ergonomics 54:34–46

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Terao Y, Andersson NE, Flanagan JR, Johansson RS (2002) Engagement of gaze in capturing targets for future sequential manual actions. J Neurophysiol 88:1716–1725

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Teulings HL, Contreras-Vidal JL, Stelmach GE, Adler CH (1997) Parkinsonism reduces coordination of fingers, wrist, and arm in fine motor control. Exp Neurol 146:159–170

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Todorov E (2004) Optimality principles in sensorimotor control. Nat Neurosci 7:907–915

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tseng P, Bridgeman B, Juan C (2012) Take the matter into your own hands: a brief review of the effect of nearby-hands on visual processing. Vis Res 72:74–77

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vesia M, Crawford JD (2012) Specialization of reach function in human posterior parietal cortex. Exp Brain Res 221:1–18

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vindras P, Desmurget M, Viviani P (2005) Error parsing in visuomotor pointing reveals independent processing of amplitude and direction. J Neurophysiol 94:1212–1224

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang W, Johnson T, Sainburg RL, Dounskaia N (2012) Interlimb differences of directional biases for stroke production. Exp Brain Res 216:263–274

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolpert DM, Diedrichsen J, Flanagan JR (2011) Principles of sensorimotor learning. Nat Rev Neurosci 12:739–751

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zito GA, Cazzoli D, Müri RM, Mosimann UP, Nef T (2016) Behavioral differences in the upper and lower visual hemifields in shape and motion perception. Front Behav Neurosci 10:128. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00128.eCollection2016

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by Grant Ra 2183/1-3 of the German Research Foundation (DFG). We thank Anika Beyer for her support in data collection.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Miya K. Rand.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Appendix

Appendix

Assessments of adaptive changes, after-effects, and explicit knowledge

The participants underwent two types of pre-tests and three types of post-tests before and after the practice, respectively. Procedures of these tests were described in the main text (Table 1). All these tests were performed without online feedback, thereby excluding any changes attributed to the online visual feedback control. Relative changes of reaching direction between pre- and post-tests were used to assess the magnitudes of adaptive changes (adaptive shift), after-effects, and explicit knowledge (explicit shift) that reflected different types of knowledge of the visuomotor rotation acquired through practice (Heuer et al. 2013; Rentsch and Rand 2014; Sülzenbrück and Heuer 2011). The details of these assessments were similar to those described in our previous study (Rentsch and Rand 2014).

For data analysis, angular deviation of a vector (from the hand position at movement onset to that at movement offset) from a SP-target vector (from the starting position to the target) was computed in each trial of the pre-test 1, post-test 1 and post-test 2. In each trial of explicit pre-test and explicit post-test, angular deviation of the judged direction of the line from the direction of the SP-target vector was computed. Next, a circular mean (Berens 2009) of angular deviations across the first three trials of each test was calculated in each target direction and participant. Then, the difference between the circular mean of post-test 1 (or post-test 2) and that of pre-test 1 was calculated as adaptive shift (or after-effect), and the difference between the circular mean of explicit pre-test and that of explicit post-test was calculated as explicit shift.

Adaptive shifts reflect both implicit and explicit adjustments of reaching directions based on implicit and explicit knowledge of the visuomotor rotation acquired through practice (Heuer et al. 2013). Adaptive shifts of −75° would indicate that both types of adjustments combined fully compensate for the 75° rotation. As shown in Fig. 6a, mean adaptive shifts were substantially smaller than −75°, suggesting that online feedback-based adjustments were used aside from these two types of adjustments to compensate for the rotation. A 2 (group: IDE small vs IDE large) × 4 (target direction) ANOVA with repeated measures revealed no significant main effects (p > 0.05).

Fig. 6
figure 6

Mean adaptive shifts (a), after-effects (b), and explicit shifts (c) are plotted against target directions (Fig. 1a, left plot). Mean values of all participants are plotted for the small-IDE group (filled columns) and the large-IDE group (open columns). The error bars represent standard errors

After-effects reflect only implicit adjustments of reaching directions based on implicit knowledge acquired through practice (Heuer et al. 2013). After-effects of -75° would indicate that participants acquired perfect implicit adjustments to compensate for the 75° rotation. Mean after-effects were largest for reaching to T1 and smallest to T4 (Fig. 6b). The 2 × 4 ANOVA showed a significant target direction effect [F(3,60) = 5.18, p < 0.01, η 2p  = 0.206]. A planned contrast revealed that there were significantly greater after-effects for targets related to simple joint coordination (Fig. 6b, T1 and T3; Fig. 1a, left plot in the main text) than for those related to complex coordination [T2 and T4, F(1,20) = 12.04, p < 0.01, η 2p  = 0.376]. After-effect tended to be greater for reaching to the ipsilateral targets (T1 and T2) than to the contralateral targets [T3 and T4, F(1,20) = 3.71, p = 0.068, η 2p  = 0.157]. Note that the visual feedback was veridical for both pre-test 1 and post-test 2, thereby having no distinction between the target effects of hemispace-visual stimuli and hemispace-manual actions. There were no other significant main effects (p > 0.05).

Explicit shifts reflect the magnitude of explicit knowledge of the visuomotor rotation acquired through practice (Heuer et al. 2013). Explicit shifts of −75° would indicate that participants acquired perfect explicit knowledge of the applied 75° rotation. Even though mean explicit shifts were generally greater for the small-IDE group (Fig. 6c, filled columns) than those for the large-IDE group (open columns), there was large inter-individual variability (see error bars of Fig. 6c). Consequently, group effect and target direction effect were not significant (2 × 4 ANOVA: p > 0.05), whereas a group by target interaction just failed to reach a significance [F(3,60) = 2.36, p = 0.08, η 2p  = 0.106].

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Rand, M.K., Rentsch, S. Eye–hand coordination during visuomotor adaptation: effects of hemispace and joint coordination. Exp Brain Res 235, 3645–3661 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-017-5088-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-017-5088-z

Keywords

Navigation