Abstract
Two decades of research have demonstrated that using a tool modulates spatial representations of the body. Whether this embodiment is specific to representations of the tool-using limb or extends to representations of other body parts has received little attention. Several studies of other perceptual phenomena have found that modulations to the primary somatosensory representation of the hand transfers to the face, due in part to their close proximity in primary somatosensory cortex. In the present study, we investigated whether tool-induced recalibration of tactile perception on the hand transfers to the cheek. Participants verbally estimated the distance between two tactile points applied to either their hand or face, before and after using a hand-shaped tool. Tool use recalibrated tactile distance perception on the hand—in line with previous findings—but left perception on the cheek unchanged. This finding provides support for the idea that embodiment is body-part specific. Furthermore, it suggests that tool-induced perceptual recalibration occurs at a level of somatosensory processing, where representations of the hand and face have become functionally disentangled.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.





References
Anelli F, Candini M, Cappelletti M, Oliveri M, Frassinetti F (2015) The remapping of time by active tool-use. PLoS One 10:e0146175. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0146175
Anema HA, Wolswijk VW, Ruis C, Dijkerman HC (2008) Grasping Weber’s illusion: the effect of receptor density differences on grasping and matching. Cogn Neuropsychol 25:951–967
Baccarini M, Martel M, Cardinali L, Sillan O, Farnè A, Roy AC (2014) Tool use imagery triggers tool incorporation in the body schema. Front Psychol 5:1–8
Bassolino M, Finisguerra A, Canzoneri E, Serino A, Pozzo T (2015) Dissociating effect of upper limb non-use and overuse on space and body representations. Neuropsychologia 70:385–392. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.11.028
Beck B, Bertini C, Haggard P, Ladavas E (2015) Dissociable routes for personal and interpersonal visual enhancement of touch. Cortex 73:289–297. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2015.09.008
Bourgeois J, Farnè A, Coello Y (2014) Costs and benefits of tool-use on the perception of reachable space. Acta Physiol (Oxf) 148:91–95
Brown LE, Doole R, Malfait N (2011) The role of motor learning in spatial adaptation near a tool. PLoS One 6:e28999
Calzolari E, Azañón E, Danvers M, Vallar G, Longo MR (2017) Adaptation aftereffects reveal that tactile distance is a basic somatosensory feature. Proc Natl Acad Sci 114:4555–4560
Canzoneri E, Ubaldi S, Rastelli V, Finisguerra A, Bassolino M, Serino A (2013) Tool-use reshapes the boundaries of body and peripersonal space representations. Exp Brain Res 228:25–42
Cardinali L, Frassinetti F, Brozzoli C, Urquizar C, Roy AC, Farnè A (2009) Tool-use induces morphological updating of the body schema. Curr Biol 19:R478–R479
Cardinali L, Brozzoli C, Urquizar C, Salemme R, Roy AC, Farnè A (2011) When action is not enough: tool-use reveals tactile-dependent access to body schema. Neuropsychologia 49:3750–3757. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.09.033
Cardinali L, Jacobs S, Brozzoli C, Frassinetti F, Roy A, Farnè A (2012) Grab an object with a tool and change your body: tool-use-dependent changes of body representation for action. Exp Brain Res 218:259–271. doi:10.1007/s00221-012-3028-5
Cardinali L, Brozzoli C, Finos L, Roy AC, Farnè A (2016a) The rules of tool incorporation: tool morpho-functional & sensori-motor constraints. Cognition 149:1–5. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2016.01.001
Cardinali L, Brozzoli C, Luauté J, Roy A, Farnè A (2016b) Proprioception is necessary for body schema plasticity: evidence from a deafferented patient. Front Hum Neurosci. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2016.00272
Cardini F, Longo MR, Haggard P (2011) Vision of the body modulates somatosensory intracortical inhibition. Cereb Cortex 21:2014–2022. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhq267
Costantini M, Ambrosini E, Sinigaglia C, Gallese V (2011) Tool-use observation makes far objects ready-to-hand. Neuropsychologia 49:2658–2663. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.05.013
de Vignemont F, Ehrsson HH, Haggard P (2005) Bodily illusions modulate tactile perception. Curr Biol 15:1286–1290
Fang PC, Jain N, Kaas JH (2002) Few intrinsic connections cross the hand-face border of area 3b of new world monkeys. J Comp Neurol 454:310–319. doi:10.1002/cne.10433
Farnè A, Roy AC, Giraux P, Dubernard JM, Sirigu A (2002) Face or hand, not both: perceptual correlates of reafferentation in a former amputee. Curr Biol 12:1342–1346
Farnè A, Iriki A, Là davas E (2005) Shaping multisensory action–space with tools: evidence from patients with cross-modal extinction. Neuropsychologia 43:238–248. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.11.010
Ferre ER, Vagnoni E, Haggard P (2013) Vestibular contributions to bodily awareness. Neuropsychologia 51:1445–1452. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.04.006
Gandevia SC, Phegan CM (1999) Perceptual distortions of the human body image produced by local anaesthesia, pain and cutaneous stimulation. J Physiol 514(Pt 2):609–616
Ganesh G, Yoshioka T, Osu R, Ikegami T (2014) Immediate tool incorporation processes determine human motor planning with tools. Nat Commun 5:4524. doi:10.1038/ncomms5524
Garbarini F, Fossataro C, Berti A et al (2015) When your arm becomes mine: pathological embodiment of alien limbs using tools modulates own body representation. Neuropsychologia 70:402–413. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.11.008
Giglia G, Pia L, Folegatti A et al (2015) Far space remapping by tool use: a rTMS study over the right posterior parietal cortex. Brain Stimul 8:795–800. doi:10.1016/j.brs.2015.01.412
Green BG (1982) The perception of distance and location for dual tactile pressures. Percept Psychophys 31:315–323
Haggard P, Christakou A, Serino A (2007) Viewing the body modulates tactile receptive fields. Exp Brain Res 180:187–193. doi:10.1007/s00221-007-0971-7
Halligan PW, Marshall JC, Wade DT, Davey J, Morrison D (1993) Thumb in cheek? Sensory reorganization and perceptual plasticity after limb amputation. NeuroReport 4:233–236
Hihara S, Notoya T, Tanaka M et al (2006) Extension of corticocortical afferents into the anterior bank of the intraparietal sulcus by tool-use training in adult monkeys. Neuropsychologia 44:2636–2646
Holmes NP, Calvert GA, Spence C (2007) Tool use changes multisensory interactions in seconds: evidence from the crossmodal congruency task. Exp Brain Res 183:465–476. doi:10.1007/s00221-007-1060-7
Iriki A, Tanaka M, Iwamura Y (1996) Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. NeuroReport 7:2325–2330
Jovanov K, Clifton P, Mazalek A, Nitsche M, Welsh TN (2015) The limb-specific embodiment of a tool following experience. Exp Brain Res 233(9):2685–2694
Kao K, Goodale MA (2009) Enhanced detection of visual targets on the hand and familiar tools. Neuropsychologia 47:2454–2463. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.04.016
Kennett S, Taylor-Clarke M, Haggard P (2001) Noninformative vision improves the spatial resolution of touch in humans. Curr Biol 11:1188–1191 (pii:S0960-9822(01)00327-X)
Kilteni K, Ehrsson HH (2017) Sensorimotor predictions and tool use: hand-held tools attenuate self-touch. Cognition 165:1–9. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2017.04.005
Knight LC, Longo MR, Bremner AJ (2014) Categorical perception of tactile distance. Cognition 131:254–262. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2014.01.005
Konen CS, Haggard P (2012) Multisensory parietal cortex contributes to visual enhancement of touch in humans: a single-pulse TMS study. Cereb Cortex 24(2):501–507. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhs331
Longo M, Golubova O (2017) Mapping the internal geometry of tactile space. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform (in press)
Longo MR, Haggard P (2011) Weber’s illusion and body shape: anisotropy of tactile size perception on the hand. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 37:720–726. doi:10.1037/a0021921
Longo MR, Sadibolova R (2013) Seeing the body distorts tactile size perception. Cognition 126:475–481. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.11.013
Longo MR, Pernigo S, Haggard P (2011) Vision of the body modulates processing in primary somatosensory cortex. Neurosci Lett 489:159–163. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2010.12.007
Longo MR, Ghosh A, Yahya T (2015) Bilateral symmetry of distortions of tactile size perception. Perception 44:1251–1262. doi:10.1177/0301006615594949
Makin TR, Scholz J, Filippini N, Henderson Slater D, Tracey I, Johansen-Berg H (2013) Phantom pain is associated with preserved structure and function in the former hand area. Nat Commun 4:1570. doi:10.1038/ncomms2571
Makin TR, Scholz J, Henderson Slater D, Johansen-Berg H, Tracey I (2015) Reassessing cortical reorganization in the primary sensorimotor cortex following arm amputation. Brain 138:2140–2146. doi:10.1093/brain/awv161
Mancini F, Longo MR, Iannetti GD, Haggard P (2011) A supramodal representation of the body surface. Neuropsychologia 49:1194–1201. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.040
Manger PR, Woods TM, Munoz A, Jones EG (1997) Hand/face border as a limiting boundary in the body representation in monkey somatosensory cortex. J Neurosci 17:6338–6351
Maravita A, Iriki A (2004) Tools for the body (schema). Trends Cogn Sci 8:79–86. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.12.008
Maravita A, Spence C, Driver J (2002) Tool-use changes multimodal spatial interactions between vision and touch in normal humans. Cognition 83(2):B25–B34
Martel M, Cardinali L, Roy AC, Farnè A (2016) Tool-use: an open window into body representation and its plasticity. Cogn Neuropsychol 33:82–101
Miller LE, Longo MR, Saygin AP (2014) Tool morphology constrains the effects of tools on body representations. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 40:2143–2153
Miller LE, Longo MR, Saygin AP (2016) Mental body representations retain homuncular shape distortions: evidence from Weber’s illusion. Conscious Cogn 40:17–25. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2015.12.008
Miller LE, Longo MR, Saygin AP (2017) Visual illusion of tool use recalibrates tactile perception. Cognition 162:32–40
Muret D, Dinse HR, Macchione S, Urquizar C, Farnè A, Reilly KT (2014) Touch improvement at the hand transfers to the face. Curr Biol 24:R736–R737
Muret D, Daligault S, Dinse HR, Delpuech C, Mattout J, Reilly KT, Farne A (2016) Neuromagnetic correlates of adaptive plasticity across the hand-face border in human primary somatosensory cortex. J Neurophysiol 115(4):2095–2104. doi:10.1152/jn.00628.2015
Paqueron X, Leguen M, Rosenthal D, Coriat P, Willer JC, Danziger N (2003) The phenomenology of body image distortions induced by regional anaesthesia. Brain 126:702–712
Patané I, Iachini T, Farnè A, Frassinetti F (2016) Disentangling action from social space: tool-use differently shapes the space around us. PLoS One 11:e0154247. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0154247
Patané I, Farnè A, Frassinetti F (2017) Cooperative tool-use reveals peripersonal and interpersonal spaces are dissociable. Cognition 166:13–22. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2017.04.013
Penfield W, Boldrey E (1937) Somatic motor and sensory representation in the cerebral cortex of man as studied by electrical stimulation. Brain 60:389–443
Pons TP, Garraghty PE, Ommaya AK, Kaas JH, Taub E, Mishkin M (1991) Massive cortical reorganization after sensory deafferentation in adult macaques. Science 252:1857–1860
Quallo MM, Price CJ, Ueno K, Asamizuya T, Cheng K, Lemon RN, Iriki A (2009) Gray and white matter changes associated with tool-use learning in macaque monkeys. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:18379–18384. doi:10.1073/pnas.0909751106
Rademaker RL, Wu DA, Bloem IM, Sack AT (2014) Intensive tool-practice and skillfulness facilitate the extension of body representations in humans. Neuropsychologia 56:196–203. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.01.011
Ramachandran VS, Rogers-Ramachandran D (1996) Synaesthesia in phantom limbs induced with mirrors. Proc Biol Sci 263:377–386. doi:10.1098/rspb.1996.0058
Ramachandran VS, Stewart M, Rogers-Ramachandran D (1992) Perceptual correlates of massive cortical reorganization. NeuroReport 3:583–586
Schaefer M, Rothemund Y, Heinze HJ, Rotte M (2004) Short-term plasticity of the primary somatosensory cortex during tool use. NeuroReport 15:1293–1297 (pii:00001756-200406070-00015)
Sengül A, Rognini G, van Elk M, Aspell JE, Bleuler H, Blanke O (2013) Force feedback facilitates multisensory integration during robotic tool use. Exp Brain Res 227:497–507
Serino A, Bassolino M, Farnè A, Là davas E (2007) Extended multisensory space in blind cane users. Psychol Sci 18:642–648. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01952.x
Serino A, Padiglioni S, Haggard P, Ladavas E (2009) Seeing the hand boosts feeling on the cheek. Cortex 45:602–609. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2008.03.008
Serino A, Canzoneri E, Marzolla M, di Pellegrino G, Magosso E (2015) Extending peripersonal space representation without tool-use: evidence from a combined behavioral-computational approach. Front Behav Neurosci 9:4. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00004
Spitoni GF, Galati G, Antonucci G, Haggard P, Pizzamiglio L (2010) Two forms of touch perception in the human brain. Exp Brain Res 207:185–195. doi:10.1007/s00221-010-2446-5
Spitoni GF, Pireddu G, Cimmino RL et al (2013) Right but not left angular gyrus modulates the metric component of the mental body representation: a tDCS study. Exp Brain Res 228:63–72
Sposito A, Bolognini N, Vallar G, Maravita A (2012) Extension of perceived arm length following tool-use: clues to plasticity of body metrics. Neuropsychologia 50:2187–2194. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.05.022
Tajadura-Jiménez A, Väljamäe A, Toshima I, Kimura T, Tsakiris M, Kitagawa N (2012) Action sounds recalibrate perceived tactile distance. Curr Biol 22:R516–R517. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2012.04.028
Tajadura-Jiménez A, Tsakiris M, Marquardt T, Bianchi-Berthouze N (2015) Action sounds update the mental representation of arm dimension: contributions of kinaesthesia and agency. Front Psychol 6:689. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00689
Taylor-Clarke M, Jacobsen P, Haggard P (2004) Keeping the world a constant size: object constancy in human touch. Nat Neurosci 7:219–220. doi:10.1038/nn1199
Weiss T, Miltner WH, Liepert J, Meissner W, Taub E (2004) Rapid functional plasticity in the primary somatomotor cortex and perceptual changes after nerve block. Eur J Neurosci 20:3413–3423. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03790.x
Witt JK, Proffitt DR, Epstein W (2005) Tool use affects perceived distance, but only when you intend to use it. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 31:880–888. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.31.5.880
Yamamoto S, Kitazawa S (2001) Sensation at the tips of invisible tools. Nat Neurosci 4:979–980
Yang T, Gallen C, Schwartz B, Bloom F (1993) Noninvasive somatosensory homunculus mapping in humans by using a large-array biomagnetometer. Proc Natl Acad Sci 90:3098–3102
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by the Kavli Institute for Brain and Mind, UCSD. LM was additionally supported by an NIMH training grant from the Institute for Neural Computation, UCSD; APS by NSF (CAREER BCS1151805) and DARPA; and MRL by European Research Council Grant ERC-StG-336050 under the FP7.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
LM designed the experiment, analyzed the data, and wrote the paper. ACB collected and analyzed the data. MRL supervised the project and wrote the paper. APS supervised the project and wrote the paper.
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Miller, L.E., Cawley-Bennett, A., Longo, M.R. et al. The recalibration of tactile perception during tool use is body-part specific. Exp Brain Res 235, 2917–2926 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-017-5028-y
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-017-5028-y