Abstract
Action competency is defined as the ability of an individual to self-evaluate their own performance capabilities. The current experiment demonstrated that physical and observational training with a motor skill alters action competency ratings in a similar manner. Using a pre-test and post-test protocol, the results revealed that action competency is constrained prior to training by the intrinsic dynamics of relative phase (ϕ), with in-phase (ϕ = 0°) and anti-phase (ϕ = 180°) patterns receiving higher competency ratings than other relative phase patterns. After 2 days of training, action competency ratings for two trained relative phase patterns, +60° and +120°, increased following physical practice or observational practice. A transfer test revealed that both physical performance ability and action competency ability transferred to the symmetry partners (−60° and −120°) of the two trained relative phase patterns following physical or observational training. The findings also revealed that relative motion direction acts as categorical information that helps to organize action production and facilitate action competency. The results are interpreted based on the coordination dynamics theory of perception–action coupling, and extend this theory by showing that visual perception, action production, and action competency are all constrained in a consistent manner by the dynamics of the order parameter relative phase. As a whole, the findings revealed that relative motion, relative phase, and possibly relative amplitude information are all distinct sources of information that contribute to the emergence of a kinematic understanding of action in the nervous system.
Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
The 0° in-phase pattern has been described as two components moving in the same relative motion direction throughout a cycle, whereas the 180° anti-phase pattern has been described as two components moving in completely opposite directions at all times throughout a cycle (Wilson et al. 2005). Here, both of these patterns are classified as neutral with regard to relative motion direction because there is no lead or lag of one component over the other during the cycle, a distinction not shared with the other 10 relative phase animations.
References
Amazeen PG (2002) Is dynamics the content of a generalized motor program for rhythmic interlimb coordination. J Mot Behav 34:233–251
Bandura A (1986) Social Foundations of thought and action: a social cognitive theory. Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs
Bertenthal BI, Pinto J (1993) Complementary processes in the perception and production of human movements. In: Smith LB, Thelen E (eds) A dynamic systems approach to development: applications. Bradford Books, Cambridge, pp 209–239
Bertenthal BI, Pinto J (1994) Global processing of biological motions. Psychol Sci 5:221–225
Bingham GP (2004) A perceptually driven dynamical model of bimanual rhythmic movement (and phase perception). Ecol Psychol 16:45–53
Bingham GP, Schmidt RC, Zaal FTJM (1999) Visual perception of the relative phasing of human limb movements. Percept Psychophys 61:246–258
Bingham GP, Zaal F, Shull JA, Collins DR (2001) The effect of frequency on the visual perception of relative phase and phase variability of two oscillating objects. Exp Brain Res 136:543–552. doi:10.1007/s002210000610
Buchanan JJ (2004) Learning a single limb multi-joint coordination pattern: the impact of a mechanical constraint on the coordination dynamics of learning and transfer. Exp Brain Res 156:39–54
Buchanan JJ, Dean N (2014) Consistently modeling the same movement strategy is more important than model skill level in observational learning contexts. Acta Psychol 146:19–27
Buchanan JJ, Kelso JAS (1993) Posturally induced transitions in rhythmic multijoint limb movements. Exp Brain Res 94:131–142
Buchanan JJ, Wright DL (2011) Generalization of action knowledge following observational learning. Acta Psychol 136:167–178
Buchanan JJ, Kelso JAS, de Guzman GC (1997) Self-organization of trajectory information I. Experimental evidence. Biol Cybern 76:257–273
Buchanan JJ, Ryu YU, Zihlman K, Wright DA (2008) Observational practice of a relative phase pattern but not an amplitude ratio in a multijoint task. Exp Brain Res 191:157–169
Burke CJ, Tobler PN, Baddeley M, Schultz W (2010) Neural mechanisms of observational learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107:14431–14436. doi:10.1073/pnas.1003111107
Calvo-Merino B, Ehrenberg S, Leung D, Haggard P (2010) Experts see it all: configural effects in action observation. Psychol Res 74:400–406. doi:10.1007/s00426-009-0262-y
Carroll WR, Bandura A (1982) The role of visual monitoring in observational learning of action patterns: making the unobservable observable. J Mot Behav 14:153–167
Carson RG, Riek S (1998) The influence of joint position on the dynamics of perception–action coupling. Exp Brain Res 121:103–114
Casile A, Giese MA (2006) Nonvisual motor training influences biological motion perception. Curr Biol 16:69–74. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2005.10.071
Castiello U (2003) Understanding other people’s actions: intention and attention. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 29:416–430. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.29.2.416
Clark S, Tremblay F, Ste-Marie D (2004) Differential modulation of corticospinal excitability during observation, mental imagery and imitation of hand actions. Neuropsychologia 42:105–112
Cross ES, Hamilton A, Grafton ST (2006) Building a motor simulation de novo: observation of dance by dancers. Neuroimage 31:1257–1267. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.033
Cross ES, Kraemer DJM, Hamilton AFD, Kelley WM, Grafton ST (2009) Sensitivity of the action observation network to physical and observational learning. Cereb Cortex 19:315–326
de Guzman GC, Kelso JAS, Buchanan JJ (1997) Self-organization of trajectory formation II. Theoretical Model. Biol Cybern 76:275–284
Decety J, Grezes J, Costes N et al (1997) Brain activity during observation of actions—influence of action content and subject’s strategy. Brain 120:1763–1777
Fogassi L, Ferrari PF, Gesierich B, Rozzi S, Chersi F, Rizzolatti G (2005) Parietal lobe: from action organization to intention understanding. Science 308:662–667
Fontaine RJ, Lee TD, Swinnen SP (1997) Learning a new bimanual coordination pattern: reciprocal influences of intrinsic and to-be-learned patterns. Can J Exp Psychol 51:1–9
Gibson JJ, Gibson EJ (1955) Perceptual learning—differentiation or enrichment. Psychol Rev 62:32–41
Haken H (1983) Synergetics. Springer, Berlin
Haken H, Kelso JAS, Bunz H (1985) A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements. Biol Cybern 51:347–356
Haken H, Kelso JAS, Fuchs A, Pandya AS (1990) Dynamic pattern-recognition of coordinated biological motion. Neural Netw 3:395–401. doi:10.1016/0893-6080(90)90022-d
Haken H, Peper CE, Beek PJ, Daffertshofer A (1996) A model for phase transitions in human hand movements during multifrequency tapping. Phys D 90:179–196
Hamilton A, Grafton ST (2007) The motor hierarchy: from kinematics to goals and intentions. In: Haggard P, Rossetti Y, Kawato M (eds) Sensorimotor foundations of higher cognition. Attention and performance XXll. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 381–408
Hodges NJ, Franks IM (2000) Attention focusing instructions and coordination bias: implications for learning a novel bimanual task. Hum Mov Sci 19:843–867
Hodges NJ, Chua R, Franks IM (2003) The role of video in facilitating perception and action of a novel coordination movement. J Mot Behav 35:247–260
Hommel B, Musseler J, Aschersleben G, Prinz W (2001) The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behav Brain Sci 24:849. doi:10.1017/s0140525x01000103
Jirsa VK, Kelso JAS (eds) (2004) Coordination dynamics: issues and trends. Springer, Berlin
Johansson G (1973) Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Percept Psychophys 14:201–211
Kagerer FA, Summers JJ, Semjen A (2003) Instabilities during antiphase bimanual movements: are ipsilateral pathways involved? Exp Brain Res 151:489–500
Kelso JAS (1984) Phase transitions and critical behavior in human bimanual coordination. Am J Physiol 15:R1000–R1004
Kelso JAS (1994) The informational character of self-organized coordination dynamics. Hum Mov Sci 13:393–413
Kelso JAS (1995) Dynamic patterns: the self-organization of brain and behavior. The MIT Press, Cambridge
Kelso JAS, Pandya AS (1991) Dynamic pattern generation and recognition. In: Badler NI, Barsky BA, Zeltzer D (eds) Making them move: mechanics, control, and animation of articulated figures. Morgan Kaufmann, San Mateo, pp 171–190
Kelso JAS, Scholz JP, Schoner G (1986) Nonequilibrium phase-transitions in coordinated biological motion: critical fluctuations. Phys Lett A 118:279–284. doi:10.1016/0375-9601(86)90359-2
Kelso JAS, Buchanan JJ, Wallace SA (1991) Order parameters for the neural organization of single limb, multijoint movement patterns. Exp Brain Res 85:432–445
Kennerley SW, Diedrichsen J, Hazeltine E, Semjen A, Ivry B (2002) Callosotomy patients exhibit temporal uncoupling during continuous bimanual movements. Nat Neurosci 5:376–381
Kilner JM, Friston KJ, Frith CD (2007) The mirror-neuron system: a Bayesian perspective. NeuroReport 18:619–623. doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e3281139ed0
Kostrubiec V, Tallet J, Zanone PG (2006) How a new behavioral pattern is stabilized with learning determines its persistence and flexibility in memory. Exp Brain Res 170:238–244. doi:10.1007/s00221-005-0208-6
Kovacs AJ, Buchanan JJ, Shea CH (2009) Bimanual 1:1 with 90° continuous relative phase: difficult or easy! Exp Brain Res 193:129–136. doi:10.1007/s00221-008-1676-2
Kovacs AJ, Buchanan JJ, Shea CH (2010) Perceptual and attentional influences on continuous 2:1 and 3:2 multi-frequency bimanual coordination. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 36:936–954
Lago A, Fernandez-del-Olmo M (2011) Movement observation specifies motor programs activated by the action observed objective. Neurosci Lett 493:102–106. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2011.02.014
Lago-Rodriguez A, Lopez-Alonso V, Fernández-del-Olmo M (2013) Mirror neuron system and observational learning: behavioral and neurophysiological evidence. Behav Brain Res 248:104–113. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2013.03.033
Lee TD, Swinnen SP, Verschueren S (1995) Relative phase alterations during bimanual skill acquisition. J Mot Behav 27:263–274
Liao C-M, Masters RSW (2001) Analogy learning: a means to implicit motor learning. J Sports Sci 19:307–319
Maslovat D, Hodges NJ, Krigolson OE, Handy TC (2010) Observational practice benefits are limited to perceptual improvements in the acquisition of a novel coordination skill. Exp Brain Res 204:119–130. doi:10.1007/s00221-010-2302-7
Maxwell JP, Masters RSW, Eves FF (1999) From novice to no know-how: a longitudinal study of implicit motor learning. J Sports Sci 18:111–120
Mazzoni P, Krakauer JW (2006) An implicit plan overrides an explicit strategy during visuomotor adaptation. J Neurosci 26:3642–3645
Mechsner F, Knoblich G (2004) Do muscles matter for coordinated action? J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 30:490–503
Mechsner F, Kerzel D, Knobllch G, Prinz W (2001) Perceptual basis of bimanual coordination. Nature 414:69–73
Moon H, Robson NP, Langari R, Buchanan JJ (2012) Experimental observations on the central nervous system’s governing strategies on the arm reaching with reduced mobility. In: ASME 2012 international mechanical engineering congress and exposition, vol 2: biomedical and biotechnology, Houston, TX, pp 483–492
Pinto J, Bertenthal BI (1992) Effects of phase relations on the perception of biomechanical motions. In: Association for research in vision and ophthalmology, vol 33. Investigative ophthalmology and visual science, Sarasota, FL, p 1114
Salesse R, Temprado JJ, Swinnen SP (2005) Interaction of neuromuscular, spatial and visual constraints on hand-foot coordination dynamics. Hum Mov Sci 24:66–80. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2004.12.002
Schöner G (1989) Learning and recall in a dynamic theory of coordination patterns. Biol Cybern 62:29–54
Schöner G, Kelso JAS (1988a) Dynamic pattern generation in behavioral and neural systems. Science 239:1513–1520
Schöner G, Kelso JAS (1988b) A synergetic theory of environmentally-specified and learned patterns of movement coordination. 2. component oscillator dynamics. Biol Cybern 58:81–89. doi:10.1007/bf00364154
Schöner G, Kelso JAS (1988c) A synergetic theory of environmentally-specified and learned patterns of movement coordination. I. Relative phase dynamics. Biol Cybern 58:71–80
Schöner G, Haken H, Kelso JAS (1986) A stochastic theory of phase transitions in human hand movement. Biol Cybern 53:247–257
Schöner G, Zanone PG, Kelso JAS (1992) Learning as change of coordination dynamics: theory and experiment. J Mot Behav 24:29–48
Smethurst CJ, Carson RG (2001) The acquisition of movement skills: practice enhances the dynamic stability of bimanual coordination. Hum Mov Sci 20:499–529. doi:10.1016/s0167-9457(01)00065-3
Snapp-Childs W, Wilson AD, Bingham GP (2011) The stability of rhythmic movement coordination depends on relative speed: the Bingham model supported. Exp Brain Res 215:89–100. doi:10.1007/s00221-011-2874-x
Summers JJ, Davis AS, Byblow WD (2002) The acquisition of bimanual coordination is mediated by anisotropic coupling between the hands. Hum Mov Sci 21:699–721
Tallet J, Kostrubiec V, Zanone PG (2008) The role of stability in the dynamics of learning, memorizing, and forgetting new coordination patterns. J Mot Behav 40:103–116
Temprado JJ, Salesse R, Summers JJ (2007) Neuromuscular and spatial constraints on bimanual hand-held pendulum oscillations: dissociation or combination? Hum Mov Sci 26:235–246. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2007.01.012
Tuller B, Kelso JAS (1989) Environmentally-specified patterns of movement coordination in normal and split-brain subjects. Exp Brain Res 75:306–316
Wenderoth N, Bock O, Krohn R (2002) Learning a new bimanual coordination pattern is influenced by existing attractors. Mot Cont 6:166–182
Wilson AD, Collins DR, Bingham GP (2005) Human movement coordination implicates relative direction as the information for relative phase. Exp Brain Res 165:351–361
Wilson AD, Snapp-Childs W, Bingham GP (2010a) Perceptual learning immediately yields new stable motor coordination. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 36:1508–1514. doi:10.1037/a0020412
Wilson AD, Snapp-Childs W, Coats R, Bingham GP (2010b) Learning a coordinated rhythmic movement with task-appropriate coordination feedback. Exp Brain Res 205:513–520. doi:10.1007/s00221-010-2388-y
Yamanishi J, Kawato M, Suzuki R (1980) 2 Coupled oscillators as a model for the coordinated finger tapping by both hands. Biol Cybern 37:219–225
Zaal FTJM, Bingham GP, Schmidt RC (2000) Visual perception of mean relative phase and phase variability. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 26:1209–1220
Zanone PG, Kelso JAS (1992) The evolution of behavioral attractors with learning: nonequilibrium phase transitions. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 18:403–421
Zanone PG, Kelso JAS (1997) Coordination dynamics of learning and transfer: collective and component levels. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 23:1454–1480
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Electronic supplementary material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Buchanan, J.J., Ramos, J. & Robson, N. The perception–action dynamics of action competency are altered by both physical and observational training. Exp Brain Res 233, 1289–1305 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-015-4207-y
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-015-4207-y