Skip to main content
Log in

Concurrent adaptation of reactive saccades and hand pointing movements to equal and to opposite changes of target direction

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Eye as well as hand movements can adapt to double-step target displacements, but it is still controversial whether both motor systems use common or distinct adaptive mechanisms. Here, we posit that analyses of the concurrent adaptation of both motor systems to equal versus different double-steps may provide more conclusive evidence than previous work about the transfer of adaptation from one motor system to the other. Forty subjects adapted to double-steps which called for a change of response direction. The same (group S) or the opposite change (group O) was required for eyes and hand. Group ON equaled O, except that no visual feedback of the hand was provided. Groups E and H served as controls for eyes-only and hand-only adaptation, respectively. We found no differences between groups or motor systems when comparing S, E and H. Adaptation was faster in O than in S, E and H, and faster still in ON. However, the magnitude of eye adaptation was much smaller in O and ON than in S, E and H. We conclude that concurrent adaptation of eye and hand directions to opposite double-steps attenuates recalibration which, at least for the hand, is largely replaced by workaround strategies. The mechanisms for eye and hand adaptation therefore seem to be coupled, in a way that hinders divergent recalibration of both motor systems. The possible neuronal substrate for our findings is discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Saccade direction was defined, in analogy to pointing direction, as angular difference between primary target and saccade direction.

  2. We thought five episodes were enough to estimate whether eye and hand adaptation differed.

  3. The registered eye movements consisted of a primary saccade typically followed in double-step trials by a corrective saccade. Here, we analyze only the reaction time and direction of primary saccades.

  4. The respective ANOVA outcomes were (F(29,377) = 5.68, p < 0.001); (F(29,377) = 4.6, p < 0.001); (F(29,377) = 5.4, p < 0.001) and (F(29,377) = 5.12, p < 0.001).

References

  • Alahyane N, Koene A, Pélisson D (2004) Transfer of adaptation from visually guided saccades to averaging saccades elicited by double visual targets. Eur J Neurosci 20:827–836

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alahyane N, Salemme R, Urquizar C, Cotti J, Guillaume A, Vercher J-L et al (2007) Oculomotor plasticity: are mechanisms of adaptation for reactive and voluntary saccades separate? Brain Res 1135:107–121

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Andersen RA, Buneo CA (2002) Intentional maps in posterior parietal cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 25:189–220

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Asanuma C, Andersen RA, Cowan WM (1985) The thalamic relations of the caudal inferior parietal lobule and the lateral prefrontal cortex in monkeys: divergent cortical projections from cell clusters in the medial pulvinar nucleus. J Comp Neurol 241:357–381

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Becker W, Jürgens R (1979) An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli. Vis Res 19:967–983

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Biguer B, Jeannerod M, Prablanc C (1982) The coordination of eye, head, and arm movements during reaching at a single visual target. Exp Brain Res 46:301–304

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bock O (2005) Components of sensorimotor adaptation in young and elderly subjects. Exp Brain Res 160:259–263

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bock O, Abeele S, Eversheim U (2003) Human adaptation to rotated vision: interplay of a continuous and a discrete process. Exp Brain Res 152:528–532

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bock O, Worringham C, Thomas M (2005) Concurrent adaptations of left and right arms to opposite visual distortions. Exp Brain Res 162:513–519

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bock O, Schmitz G, Grigorova V (2008) Transfer of adaptation between ocular saccades and arm movements. Hum Mov Sci 27:383–395

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clower D, Boussaoud D (2000) Selective use of perceptual recalibration versus visuomotor skill acquisition. J Neurophysiol 84:2703–2708

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Clower DM, Hoffman JM, Votaw JR, Faber TL, Woods RP, Alexander GE (1996) Role of posterior parietal cortex in the recalibration of visually guided reaching. Nature 383:618–621

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cotti J, Guillaume A, Alahyane N, Pelisson D, Vercher JL (2007) Adaptation of voluntary saccades, but not of reactive saccades, transfers to hand pointing movements. J Neurophysiol 98:602–612

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Courjon JH, Olivier E, Pélisson D (2004) Direct evidence for the contribution of the superior colliculus in the control of visually guided reaching movements in the cat. J Physiol 556:675–681

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Desmurget M, Pélisson D, Urquizar C, Prablanc C, Alexander GE, Grafton ST (1998) Functional anatomy of saccadic adaptation in humans. Nat Neurosci 1:524–528

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Desmurget M, Pélisson D et al (2000) Functional adaptation of reactive saccades in humans: a PET study. Exp Brain Res 132:243–259

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Deubel H (1987) Adaptivity of gain and direction in oblique saccades. In: O’Regan J, Levy-Schoen A (eds) Eye movements: from physiology to cognition. North Holland, Amsterdam, pp 181–191

    Google Scholar 

  • Deubel H (1995) Separate adaptive mechanisms for the control of reactive and volitional saccadic eye-movements. Vis Res 35:3529–3540

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frens MA, Van Opstal AJ (1994) Transfer of short-term adaptation in human saccadic eye-movements. Exp Brain Res 100:293–306

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frens MA, Van Opstal AJ (1997) Monkey superior colliculus activity during short-term saccadic adaptation. Brain Res Bull 43:473–483

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gielen CC, van Asten WN (1990) Postural responses to simulated moving environments are not invariant for the direction of gaze. Exp Brain Res 79:167–174

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Girgenrath M, Bock O, Seitz RJ (2008) An fMRI study of brain activation in a visual adaptation task: activation limited to sensory guidance. Exp Brain Res 184:561–569

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kröller J, De Graaf JB, Prablanc C, Pelisson D (1999) Effects of short-term adaptation of saccadic gaze amplitude on hand-pointing movements. Exp Brain Res 124:351–362

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lazar G, Van Laer J (1968) Adaptation to displaced vision after experience with lesser displacements. Percept Mot Scills 26:579–582

    Google Scholar 

  • Lünenburger L, Kutz DF, Hoffmann KP (2000) Influence of arm movements on saccades in humans. Eur J Neurosci 12:4107–4116

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Magescas F, Prablanc C (2006) Automatic drive of limb motor plasticity. J Cogn Neurosci 18:75–83

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McLaughlin SC (1967) Parametric adjustment in saccadic eye movements. Percept Psychophys 2:359–362

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miall RC, Reckess GZ, Imamizu H (2001) The cerebellum coordinates eye and hand tracking movements. Nat Neurosci 4:638–644

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Miall RC, Jenkinson N, Kulkarni K (2004) Adaptation to rotated visual feedback: a re-examination of motor interference. Exp Brain Res 154:201–210

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neggers SF, Bekkering H (2002) Coordinated control of eye and hand movements in dynamic reaching. Hum Mov Sci 21:349–376

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Niechwiej-Szwedo E, McIlroy WE, Green R, Verrier MC (2005) The effect of directional compatibility on the response latencies of ocular and manual movements. Exp Brain Res 162:220–229

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Noto CT, Watanabe S, Fuchs AF (1999) Characteristics of simian adaptation fields produced by behavioral changes in saccade size and direction. J Neurophysiol 81:2798–2813

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pélisson D, Prablanc C, Goodale MA, Jeannerod M (1986) Visual control of reaching movements without vision of the limb. II. Evidence of fast unconscious processes correcting the trajectory of the hand to the final position of a double-step stimulus. Exp Brain Res 62:303–311

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pesaran B, Nelson MJ, Andersen RA (2006a) Dorsal premotor neurons encode the relative position of the hand, eye, and goal during reach planning. Neuron 51:125–134

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pesaran B, Musallam S, Andersen RA (2006b) Cognitive neural prosthetics. Curr Biol 16:R77–R80

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Redding GM, Wallace B (1996) Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptual motor control. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 22:379–394

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitz G, Bock O, Grigorova V, Ilieva M (2010) Adaptation of eye and hand movements to target displacements of different size. Exp Brain Res 203:479–484

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seidler RD, Bloomberg JJ, Stelmach GE (2001) Context-dependent arm pointing adaptation. Behav Brain Res 119:155–166

    Google Scholar 

  • Snyder LH, Batista AP, Andersen RA (2000) Saccade-related activity in the parietal reach region. J Neurophysiol 83:1099–1102

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Snyder LH, Calton JL, Dickinson AR, Lawrence BM (2002) Eye-hand coordination: saccades are faster when accompanied by a coordinated arm movement. J Neurophysiol 87:2279–2286

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Straube A, Fuchs AF, Usher S, Robinson FR (1997) Characteristics of saccadic gain adaptation in rhesus macaques. J Neurophysiol 77:874–895

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stuphorn V, Bauswein E, Hoffmann KP (2000) Neurons in the primate superior colliculus coding for arm movements in gaze-related coordinates. J Neurophysiol 83:1283–1299

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Takeichi N, Kaneko CRS, Fuchs AF (2007) Activity changes in monkey superior colliculus during saccade adaptation. J Neurophysiol 97:4096–4107

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas M, Bock O (2010) Is sensorimotor adaptation to position- and velocity-dependent visual distortions based on distinct adaptive processes? Hum Mov Sci 29:179–186

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas M, Bock O (2012) Concurrent adaptation to four different visual rotations. Exp Brain Res 221:85–91

    Google Scholar 

  • Tipper SP, Howard LA, Paul MA (2001) Reaching affects saccade trajectories. Exp Brain Res 136:241–249

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • van Sonderen JF, Denier van der Gon JJ, Gielen CCAM (1988) Conditions determining early modification of motor programmes in response to changes in target location. Exp Brain Res 71:320–328

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vercher JL, Magenes G, Prablanc C, Gauthier GM (1994) Eye-head-hand coordination in pointing at visual targets: spatial and temporal analysis. Exp Brain Res 99:507–523

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wang J, Sainburg RL (2003) Mechanisms underlying interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations. Exp Brain Res 149:520–526

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Werner W (1993) Neurons in the primate superior colliculus are active before and during arm movements to visual targets. Eur J Neurosci 5:335–340

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Werner W, Dannenberg S, Hoffmann KP (1997a) Arm-movement-related neurons in the primate superior colliculus and underlying reticular formation: comparison of neuronal activity with EMGs of muscles of the shoulder, arm and trunk during reaching. Exp Brain Res 115:191–205

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Werner W, Hoffmann KP, Dannenberg S (1997b) Anatomical distribution of arm-movement-related neurons in the primate superior colliculus and underlying reticular formation in comparison with visual and saccadic cells. Exp Brain Res 115:206–216

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wigmore V, Tong C, Flanagan JR (2002) Visuomotor rotations of varying size and direction compete for a single internal model in motor working memory. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 28:447–457

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by DFG exchange grant 436BUL113/148/0-1 and by DFG grant BO 649/8-5.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Valentina Grigorova.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Grigorova, V., Bock, O. & Borisova, S. Concurrent adaptation of reactive saccades and hand pointing movements to equal and to opposite changes of target direction. Exp Brain Res 226, 63–71 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3411-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3411-x

Keywords

Navigation