Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Exploring the relationship between boredom and sustained attention

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Boredom is a common experience, prevalent in neurological and psychiatric populations, yet its cognitive characteristics remain poorly understood. We explored the relationship between boredom proneness, sustained attention and adult symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). The results showed that high boredom-prone individuals (HBP) performed poorly on measures of sustained attention and showed increased symptoms of ADHD and depression. The results also showed that HBP individuals can be characterised as either apathetic—in which the individual is unconcerned with his/her environment, or as agitated—in which the individual is motivated to engage in meaningful activities, although attempts to do so fail to satisfy. Apathetic boredom proneness was associated with attention lapses, whereas agitated boredom proneness was associated with decreased sensitivity to errors of sustained attention, and increased symptoms of adult ADHD. Our results suggest there is a complex relationship between attention and boredom proneness.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We could have chosen the 2-factor structure suggested by Ahmed (1990) or the short-form 2-factor structure used by Vodanovich et al. (2005). We chose the 5-factor structure for two reasons: first, a previous data set (Goldberg et al. 2011) had confirmed the existence of a 5-factor structure in a larger sample (n = 823). Second, when we split the group based on Ahmed's (1990) structure, no participant changed group (i.e., the individuals identified as experiencing apathetic or agitated boredom proneness by the 5-factor structure were the same individuals identified by the 2-factor structure).

References

  • Ahmed SMS (1990) Psychometric properties of the Boredom Proneness Scale. Percept Mot Skills 71:963–966

    Google Scholar 

  • Antony MM, Bieling PJ, Cox BJ, Murray WE, Swinson RP (1998) Psychometric properties of the 42-item and 21-item versions of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales in clinical groups and community sample. Psych Assess 2:176–181

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barkley RA (2006) The nature of ADHD: attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. A handbook for diagnosis and treatment, 3rd edn. Guilford Publications, NY

    Google Scholar 

  • Bracke P, Verhaeghe M (2010) Structural determinants of boredom among the clients of psychosocial and vocational rehabilitation centers. J Appl Soc Psych 40:1969–1998

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown KW, Ryan RM (2003) The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J Pers Soc Psych 84:822–848

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carriere JSA, Cheyne JA, Smilek D (2008) Everyday attention lapses and memory failures: the affective consequences of mindlessness. Conscious Cogn 17:835–847

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cheyne JA, Carriere JS, Smilek D (2006) Absent-mindedness: lapses of conscious awareness and everyday cognitive failures. Conscious Cogn 15:578–592

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chisholm JD, Kingstone A (2012) Improved top-down control reduces oculomotor capture: the case of action video game players. Atten Percept Psychophys 74:257–262

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conners CK, Erhardt D, Sparrow MA (2003) Conners’ adult ADHD Rating Scales (CAARS). Arch Clin Neuropsych 18:431–437

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeCoster J (2007) Applied Linear Regression Notes set 1. Retrieved May, 1st, 2012 from http://www.stat-help.com/notes.html

  • Farmer R, Sundberg ND (1986) Boredom proneness: the development and correlates of a new scale. J Pers Assess 50:4–17

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg E, Podell K (2000) Adaptive decision making, ecological validity, and the frontal lobes. J Clin Exp Neuropsych 22:56–68

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg E, Harner R, Lovell M, Podell K, Riggio S (1994) Cognitive bias, functional cortical geometry, and the frontal lobes: laterality, sex, and handedness. J Cogn Neurosci 6:276–296

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg Y, Eastwood J, LaGuardia J, Danckert J (2011) Boredom: an emotional experience distinct from apathy, anhedonia or depression. J Soc Clin Psych 30:646–665

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenson RR (1951) Apathetic and agitated boredom. Psychoanalytic Quart 20:346–347

    Google Scholar 

  • Kass SJ, Vodanovich SJ, Callander A (2001) State-trait boredom: the relationship to absenteeism, tenure and job satisfaction. J Bus Psych 16:317–327

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kass SJ, Wallace C, Vodanovich SJ (2003) Boredom proneness and sleep disorders as predictors of adult attention deficit scores. J Att Dis 7:83–90

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kingstone A, Smilek D, Eastwood JD (2008) Cognitive ethology: a new approach for studying human cognition. Br J Psychol 99:317–340

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kooij SJ, Bejerot S, Blackwell A, Caci H, Casas-Brugue M, Carpentier DE et al (2010) European consensus statement on diagnosis and treatment of adult ADHD: the European network adult ADHD. BMC Psychiatry 10(67):1–24

    Google Scholar 

  • Kreutzer JS, Seel RT, Gourley E (2001) The prevalence and symptom rates of depression after traumatic brain injury: a comprehensive examination. Brain Inj 15:563–576

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mikulas W, Vodanovich S (1993) The essence of boredom. Psych Record 43:3–12

    Google Scholar 

  • Molenberghs P, Gillebert CR, Schoofs H, Dupont P, Peeters R, Vandenberghe R (2009) Lesion neuroanatomy of the sustained attention to response task. Neuropsychologia 47:2866–2875

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Munsterberg H (1913) Psychology and industrial efficiency. Houghton-Mifflin, Boston, Massachusetts

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • O’Hanlon JF (1981) Boredom: practical consequences of a theory. Acta Psychol 49:53–82

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pekrun R, Daniels LM, Goetz T, Stupnisky RH, Perry RP (2010) Boredom in achievement settings: exploring control-value antecedents and performance outcomes of a neglected emotion. J Educ Psych 102:531–549

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner MI (1980) Orienting of attention. Quart J Exp Psych 32:3–25

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzo M, Robin DA (1990) Simultagnosia: a defect of sustained attention yields insight on visual information processing. Neurology 40:447–455

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzo M, Robin DA (1996) Bilateral effects of unilateral visual cortex lesions in humans. Brain 19:951–963

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robertson IH, Manly T, Andrade J, Baddeley BT, Yiend J (1997) Oops!: performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia 35:747–758

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Smallwood J, Schooler JW (2006) The restless mind. Psychol Bull 132:946–958

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Todman M (2003) Boredom and psychotic disorders: cognitive and motivational issues. Psychiatry 66:146–167

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vodanovich SJ (2003) Psychometric measures of boredom: a review of the literature. J Psychol 137:569–595

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vodanovich SJ, Kass SJ (1990) A factor analytic study of the boredom proneness scale. J Pers Assess 55:115–123

    Google Scholar 

  • Vodanovich SJ, Wallace JC, Kass SJ (2005) A confirmatory approach to the factor structure of the Boredom Proneness Scale: evidence for a two-factor short form. J Pers Assess 85:295–303

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman M (1992) Dimensions of sensation seeking. J Consult Clin Psych 36:45–52

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to James Danckert.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Malkovsky, E., Merrifield, C., Goldberg, Y. et al. Exploring the relationship between boredom and sustained attention. Exp Brain Res 221, 59–67 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3147-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3147-z

Keywords

Navigation