Abstract
Directional preferences during center-out horizontal shoulder–elbow movements were previously characterized for the dominant arm. These preferences were attributed to a tendency to actively accelerate one joint, while exploiting largely passive motion at the other joint. Since the non-dominant arm is known for inefficient coordination of inter-segmental dynamics, here we hypothesized that directional preferences would differ between the arms. A center-out free-stroke drawing task was used that allowed freedom in the selection of movement directions. The task was performed both with and without a secondary cognitive task that has been shown to increase directional biases of the dominant arm. Mirror-symmetrical directional preferences were observed in both arms, with similar bias strength and secondary task effects. The preferred directions were characterized by maximal exploitation of interaction torques for movement production, but only in the dominant arm. The non-dominant arm failed to benefit from interaction torques. The results point to a hierarchical architecture of control. At the higher level, a movement capable to perform the task while satisfying preferences in joint control is specified through forward dynamic transformations. This process is mediated for both arms from a common neural network adapted to the dominant arm and, specifically, to its ability to exploit interaction torques. Dynamic transformations that determine actual control commands are specified at the lower level of control. An alternative interpretation that strokes might be planned evenly across directions, and biases emerge during movement execution due to anisotropic resistance of intrinsic factors that do not depend on arm dominance is also discussed.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Atkeson CG (1989) Learning arm kinematics and dynamics. Ann Rev Neurosci 12:157–183
Bagesteiro LB, Sainburg RL (2002) Handedness: dominant arm advantages in control of limb dynamics. J Neurophysiol 88:2408–2421
Bhushan N, Shadmehr R (1999) Computational nature of human adaptive control during learning of reaching movements in force fields. Biol Cyber 81:39–60
Bowman AW, Azzalini A (1997) Applied smoothing techniques for data analysis: the kernel approach with S-Plus illustrations. Clarendon, Oxford
Buneo CA, Boline J, Soechting JE, Poppele RE (1995) On the form of the internal model for reaching. Exp Brain Res 104:467–479
Chaffin DB, Andersson GBJ (1984) Occupational biomechanics. John Wiley & Sons, New York
Dounskaia N (2005) The internal model and the leading joint hypothesis: implications for control of multi-joint movements. Exp Brain Res 166:1–16
Dounskaia N (2010) Control of human limb movements: the leading joint hypothesis and its practical applications. Exerc Sport Sci Rev 4:201–208
Dounskaia N, Goble J (2011) The role of vision, speed and attention in overcoming directional biases during arm movements. Exp Brain Res 209:299–309
Dounskaia NV, Swinnen SP, Walter CB, Spaepen AJ, Verschueren SMP (1998) Hierarchical control of different elbow-wrist coordination patterns. Exp Brain Res 121:239–254
Dounskaia N, Ketcham CJ, Stelmach GE (2002) Commonalities and differences in control of various drawing movements. Exp Brain Res 146:11–25
Dounskaia N, Nogueira KG, Swinnen SP, Drummond E (2010) Limitations on coupling of bimanual movements caused by arm dominance: when the muscle homology principle fails. J Neurophysiol 102:2027–2038
Dounskaia N, Goble J, Wang W (2011) The role of intrinsic factors in control of arm movement direction: implications from directional preferences. J Neurophysiol 105:999–1010
Franz EA (1997) Spatial coupling in the coordination of complex actions. Q J Exp Psychol A Hum Exp Psychol 50:684–704
Georgopoulos AP, Kalaska JF, Caminiti R, Massey JT (1982) On the relations between the direction of two-dimensional arm movements and cell discharge in primate motor cortex. J Neurosci 2:1527–1537
Georgopoulos AP, Schwartz AB, Kettner RE (1986) Neuronal population coding of movement direction. Science 233(4771):1416–1419
Goble JA, Zhang Y, Shimansky Y, Sharma S, Dounskaia NV (2007) Directional biases reveal utilization of arm’s biomechanical properties for optimization of motor behavior. J Neurophysiol 98:1240–1252
Gribble PL, Ostry DJ (1999) Compensation for interaction torques during single- and multijoint limb movement. J Neurophysiol 82:2310–2326
Gritsenko V, Kalaska JF, Cisek P (2011) Descending corticospinal control of intersegmental dynamics. J Neurosci 31:11968–11979
Hollerbach JM (1982) Computers, brains, and the control of movement. Trends Neurosci 6:189–192
Izenman AJ, Sommer C (1988) Philatelic mixtures and multimodal densities. J Am Stat Assoc 83:941–953
Kelso JAS (1984) Phase transitions and critical behavior in human bimanual coordination. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 15:R1000–R1004
Koshland GE, Hasan Z (1994) Selection of muscles for initiation of planar, three-joint arm movements with different final orientations of the hand. Exp Brain Res 98:157–162
Latash ML, Aruin AS, Shapiro MB (1995) The relation between posture and movement: study of a simple synergy in a two-joint task. Hum Mov Sci 14:79–107
Miall RC, Wolpert DM (1996) Forward models for physiological motor control. Neural Netw 9:1265–1279
Minnotte MC (1997) Nonparametric testing of the existence of modes. Ann Stat 25:1646–1660
Minnotte MC, Scott DW (1993) The mode tree: a tool for visualization of nonparametric density features. J Comput Graph Stat 2:51–68
Peper CE, Beek PJ, van Wieringen PC (1995) Frequency-induced phase transitions in bimanual tapping. Biol Cybern 73:301–309
Riek S, Carson RG, Byblow WD (1992) Spatial and muscular dependencies in bimanual coordination. J Hum Mov Stud 23:251–265
Sabes PN, Jordan MI (1997) Obstacle avoidance and a perturbation sensitivity model for motor planning. J Neurosci 17:7119–7128
Sainburg RL (2002) Evidence for a dynamic-dominance hypothesis of handedness. Exp Brain Res 142:241–258
Sainburg RL (2005) Handedness: differential specializations for control of trajectory and position. Exerc Sport Sci Rev 33:206–213
Sainburg RL, Kalakanis D (2000) Differences in control of limb dynamics during dominant and nondominant arm reaching. J Neurophysiol 83:2661–2675
Schaefer SY, Haaland KY, Sainburg RL (2009) Hemispheric specialization and functional impact of ipsilesional deficits in movement coordination and accuracy. Neuropsychologia 47:2953–2966
Schwartz AB (1994) Direct cortical representation of drawing. Science 265:540–542. http://www-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov.ezproxy1.lib.asu.edu/pubmed/8036499
Scott SH, Gribble PL, Graham KM, Cabel DW (2001) Dissociation between hand motion and population vectors from neural activity in motor cortex. Nature 413:161–165
Sherwood DE (1994) Hand preference, practice order, and spatial assimilations in rapid bimanual movement. J Mot Behav 26:123–143
Swinnen SP (2002) Intermanual coordination: from behavioural principles to neural-network interactions. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:350–361
Wang J, Sainburg RL (2009) Generalization of visuomotor learning between bilateral and unilateral conditions. J Neurophysiol 102:2790–2799
Wolpert DM, Miall RC, Kerr GK, Stein JF (1993) Ocular limit cycles induced by delayed retinal feedback. Exp Brain Res 96:173–180
Acknowledgments
The study was supported by National Science Foundation (grant BCS0744747 awarded to Natalia Dounskaia).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Wang, W., Johnson, T., Sainburg, R.L. et al. Interlimb differences of directional biases for stroke production. Exp Brain Res 216, 263–274 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2927-1
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2927-1