Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 216, Issue 1, pp 51–60 | Cite as

Continuation tapping to triggered melodies: motor resonance effects of melodic motion

  • Paolo Ammirante
  • William F. ThompsonEmail author
Research Article

Abstract

Common Coding theory predicts that perceived action should resonate in produced action to which it bears some resemblance. Here we show that the qualities of motion commonly attributed to melodies are instantiated in motor plans that control timed movements. Participants attempted to tap a steady beat. Each tap triggered a sounded tone, and successive tones were systematically varied in pitch to form short melodies. Tapping behavior was monitored with motion capture. Although instructed to ignore them, triggered tones systematically affected timing and finger movement. When slower melodic motion was implied by a contour change or a smaller pitch displacement, the interval-tap interval (ITI) was longer. When faster melodic motion was implied by a preserved pitch contour or a larger pitch displacement, ITI was shorter. Kinematic recordings suggested that ITI Error arose from an initial failure to disambiguate perception (i.e., velocity implied by melodic motion) from action (i.e., finger velocity [FV]). Early in the tap trajectory, slower FV was associated with longer ITI and faster FV was associated with shorter ITI. These associations were reversed near mid-trajectory, suggesting a transition from execution of motor planning to online control (Glover et al. in Exp Brain Res 154:103–108, 2004).

Keywords

Melodic motion Continuation tapping 

References

  1. Ammirante P, Thompson WF (2010) Melodic accent as an emergent property of tonal motion. Empir Musicol Rev 5(3):94–107Google Scholar
  2. Ammirante P, Thompson WF, Russo FA (2011) Ideomotor effects of pitch in continuation tapping. Q J Exp Psychol 64(2):381–393CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Andres M, Ostry DJ, Nicol F, Paus T (2008) Time course of number magnitude interference during grasping. Cortex 44:414–419PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Barker A (1989) Greek musical writings, volume II: harmonic and acoustic theory. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  5. Baumann S, Koeneke S, Schmidt CF, Meyer M, Lutz K, Jancke L (2007) A network for audio-motor coordination in skilled pianists and non-musicians. Brain Res 1161:65–78PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Boltz M (1998) Tempo discrimination of musical patterns: effects due to pitch and rhythmic structure. Percept Psychophys 60:1357–1373PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Chapin H, Jantzen K, Kelso JA, Steinberg F, Large E (2010) Dynamic emotional and neural responses to music depend on performance expression and listener experience. PLoS One 5(12):1–14CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cohen J, Hansel CEM, Sylvester JD (1954) Interdependence of temporal and auditory judgments. Nature 174:642–644PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Crowder RG, Neath I (1994) The influence of pitch and time perception in short melodies. Music Percept 12:379–386Google Scholar
  10. Drake C, Palmer C (1993) Accent structures in music performance. Music Percept 10:343–378Google Scholar
  11. Finney SA (1997) Auditory feedback and musical keyboard performance. Music Percept 15:153–174Google Scholar
  12. Finney SA (2001) FTAP: a Linux-based program for tapping and music experiments. Behav Res Meth Instrum 33:65–72CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Flach R (2005) The transition from synchronization to continuation tapping. Hum Movement Sci 24:465–483CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Freyd JJ, Kelly MH, DeKay ML (1990) Representational momentum in memory for pitch. J Exp Psychol Learn 16(6):1107–1117Google Scholar
  15. Fujioka T, Trainor LJ, Ross B, Kakigi R, Pantev C (2004) Musical training enhances automatic encoding of melodic contour and interval structure. J Cognitive Neurosci 16:1010–1021CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Glover S, Dixon P (2001) Dynamic illusion effects in a reaching task: evidence for separate visual representations in the planning and control of reaching. J Exp Psychol Human 27:560–572CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Glover S, Dixon P (2002) Semantics affect the planning but not the control of grasping. Exp Brain Res 146:383–387PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Glover S, Rosenbaum DA, Graham J, Dixon P (2004) Grasping the meaning of words. Exp Brain Res 154:103–108PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Goebl W (2001) Melody lead in piano performance: expressive device or artifact? J Acoustic Soc Am 110:563–572CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Goebl W, Palmer C (2008) Tactile feedback and timing accuracy in piano performance. Exp Brain Res 186:471–479PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Griffiths TD, Johnsrude I, Dean JL, Green GGR (1999) A common neural substrate for the analysis of pitch and duration pattern in segmented sound? NeuroReport 18:3825–3830CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Guilford JP, Nelson HM (1936) The pitch of tones in melodies as compared with single tones. J Exp Psychol 20:309–335Google Scholar
  23. Heise GA, Miller GA (1951) An experimental study of auditory patterns. Am J Psychol 64:68–77Google Scholar
  24. Henry MJ, McAuley JD (2009) Evaluation of an imputed pitch velocity model of the auditory kappa effect. J Exp Psychol Human 35:551–564CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hommel B, Müsseler J, Aschersleben G, Prinz W (2001) The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behav Brain Sci 24:849–878PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Hubbard TL (1993) Auditory representational momentum: musical schemata and modularity. B Psychonomic Soc 31(3):201–204Google Scholar
  27. Hubbard TL (1995) Auditory representational momentum: surface form, direction, and velocity effects. Am J Psychol 108(2):255–274Google Scholar
  28. James W (1890) The principles of psychology, vol 2. Holt, New YorkCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Keele SW, Ivry RB, Pokorny RA (1987) Force control and its relation to timing. J Motor Behav 19:96–114Google Scholar
  30. Lacquiniti F, Terzuolo C, Viviani P (1983) The law relating kinematic and figural aspects of drawing movements. Acta Psychol 54:115–130CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Lee Y-S, Janata P, Frost C, Hanke M, Granger R (2011) Investigation of melodic contour processing in the brain using multivariate pattern-based fMRI. NeuroImage 57:293–300PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Leman M (2009) Embodied music cognition and mediation technology. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  33. MacKenzie N (2007) The kappa effect in pitch/time context (Doctoral dissertation, Ohio State University, 2007). Diss Abstr Int 68:132Google Scholar
  34. McAnally K (2002) Timing of finger tapping to frequency modulated acoustic stimuli. Acta Psychol 109:331–338CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Penel A, Drake C (1998) Sources of timing variations in music performance: a psychological segmentation model. Psycholog Res 61:12–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Pfordresher P, Dalla Bella S (2011) Delayed auditory feedback and movement. J Exp Psychol Human 37:566–579CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Plamondon R (1991) On the origin of asymmetric bell-shaped velocity profiles in rapid-aimed movements. In: Requin J, Stelmach GE (eds) Tutorials in motor neuroscience. Kluwer, Dortrecht, pp 283–295Google Scholar
  38. Prinz W, Aschersleben G, Koch I (2009) Cognition and action. In: Morsella E, Bargh JA, Gollwitzer PM (eds) Oxford handbook of human action. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 35–71Google Scholar
  39. Repp B (1993) Music as motion: a synopsis of Alexander Truslit’s Gestaltung und Bewegung in der Musik. Psychol Music 21:48–72CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Repp B (1996) Patterns of note onset asynchronies in expressive piano performance. J Acoustic Soc Am 100:3917–3932CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Repp B (1999) Control of expressive and metronomic timing in pianists. J Motor Behav 31:145–164CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Repp B, Keller PE (2004) Adaptation to tempo changes in sensorimotor synchronization: effects of intention, attention, and awareness. Q J Exp Psychol 57A:499–521Google Scholar
  43. Rizzolatti G, Craighero L (2004) The mirror-neuron system. Annu Rev Neurosci 27:169–192PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Rusconi E, Kwan B, Giordano BL, Umiltà C, Butterworth B (2006) Spatial representation of pitch height: the SMARC effect. Cognition 99(2):113–129PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Shigeno S (1986) The auditory tau and kappa effects for speech and nonspeech stimuli. Percept Psychophys 40:9–19PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Shigeno S (1993) The interdependence of pitch and temporal judgments by absolute pitch processors. Percept Psychophys 54:682–692PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Shove P, Repp B (1995) Music motion and performance. Theoretical and empirical perspectives. In: Rink J (ed) The practice of performance: studies in musical interpretation. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 55–83CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Stenneken P, Prinz W, Cole J, Paillard J, Aschersleben G (2006) The effect of sensory feedback on the timing of movements: evidence from deafferented patients. Brain Res 1084:123–131PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Stevens LT (1886) On the time sense. Mind 11:393–404CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Trainor LJ, Mcdonald KL, Alain C (2002) Automatic and controlled processing of melodic contour and interval information as measured by event-related brain potentials. J Cog Neuro 14:430–442CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Truslit A (1938) Gestaltung und Bewegung in der Musik. Chr. Friedrich Vieweg, BerlinGoogle Scholar
  52. Viviani P, McCollum G (1983) The relation between linear extent and velocity in drawing movements. Neuroscience 10:211–218PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Viviani P, Stucchi N (1992) Biological movements look uniform: evidence of motor-perceptual interactions. J Exp Psychol Human 18:603–623CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Wing A, Kristofferson A (1973) Response delays and the timing of discrete motor responses. Percept Psychophys 14:5–12CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Yoblick D, Salvendy G (1970) Influence of frequency on the estimation of time for auditory, visual, and tactile modalities: the kappa effect. J Exp Psychol 86:157–164PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Zatorre RJ, Mondor TA, Evans CA (1999) Auditory attention to space and frequency activates similar cerebral systems. NeuroImage 10:544–554PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Zatorre RJ, Chen JL, Penhune VB (2007) When the brain plays music: auditory-motor interactions in music perception and production. Nat Rev Neurosci 8:547–558PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Zbikowski L (1998) Metaphor and music theory. Music Theory Online 4. Retrieved from http://mto.societymusictheory.org/issues/mto.98.4.1/mto.98.4.1.zbikowski_frames.html
  59. Zwaan RA, Taylor LJ (2006) Seeing, acting, understanding: motor resonance in language comprehension. J Exp Psychol Gen 135:1–11PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Ryerson UniversityTorontoCanada
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyMacquarie UniversitySydneyAustralia

Personalised recommendations