Skip to main content
Log in

Prior information and oculomotor initiation: the effect of cues in gaps

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Eye movements reflect not only an important output of various neural control systems, but also often reflect cognitive processing. For example, saccades are frequently used as a behavioural index of attentional processing. A second important eye movement type, smooth pursuit (SP), has received much less attention in this regard. These two types of eye movement were classically thought of as being separate, but recent results have suggested a closer linkage of their control mechanisms and perhaps their interactions with cognitive processes. Prior information, in the form of cues, alters saccade latency leading to characteristic cueing effects. When the period between the appearance of the cue and the appearance of the saccade target is sufficiently long, the latency of saccades to targets appearing at cued locations is increased. This “inhibition of return” is enhanced by a second type of stimulus manipulation, the early removal of the fixation target a few hundred milliseconds before the target appears (the gap paradigm). In the current experiments, the effect of cues, and interactions between cues and long gaps were investigated. In the main pursuit experiment, and in a separate saccade experiment, subjects were presented with interleaved runs of tasks with and without long gaps (gap duration = 1 s), and with and without cues. In tasks without cues, SP latency was reduced by long gaps (mean reduction 8 ms); unexpectedly, saccade latency for non-cue tasks was increased by long gaps (mean increase 41 ms). In a control experiment with only non-cue tasks, in which SP and saccade gap and non-gap tasks were run together, SP latency was again reduced in gap tasks, while saccade latency was increased, but by much less than in the first experiment. Analysis of individual subjects’ data showed that while gaps increased saccade latency in two subjects who had participated in the main experiment (in which cues and gaps had been combined), in two naïve subjects long gaps did not affect saccade latency. In the main pursuit experiment, cues had both spatially specific and non-spatially specific (warning) effects on pursuit latency. In non-gap conditions, latency was greater when contralateral cues were presented 250 ms prior to the appearance of the pursuit target, compared to ipsilateral cues, a pattern of effect consistent with inhibition of return. However, this was reversed when cues appeared during a gap—contralateral cues increased while ipsilateral cues decreased latency. For saccades, as expected, in both gap and non-gap conditions, cue effects were consistent with inhibition of return (latency was lower with contralateral cues), and the inhibition of return effect was larger in gap, compared to non-gap conditions. The results suggest that, in appropriate contexts (or as a result of appropriate training), there are distinct inhibitory mechanisms that operate on saccades but not pursuit. What appears to be an inhibition of return effect on pursuit latency when static cues are presented in pursuit tasks, may be better understood as the product of a modulation of mechanisms active in pursuit initiation, perhaps related to motion processing. In contrast to some recent evidence suggesting a close anatomical and functional linkage between pursuit and saccade initiation, the results are consistent with the involvement of a wider range of mechanisms, or a greater degree of flexibility, in programming the initiation of these two oculomotor behaviours.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abrams RA, Christ SE (2003) Motion onset captures attention. Psychol Sci 14:427–432

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Abrams RA, Christ SE (2005) Onset but not offset of irrelevant motion disrupts inhibition of return. Percept Psychophys 67:1460–1467

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Abrams R, Dobkin R (1994) The gap effect and inhibition of return: interactive effects on eye movement latencies. Exp. Brain Res 98:483–487

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Adler SA, Bala J, Krauzlis RJ (2002) Primacy of spatial information in guiding target selection for pursuit and saccades. J Vis 2:627–644

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boot WR, Kramer AF, Peterson MS (2005) Oculomotor consequences of abrupt object onsets and offsets: onsets dominate oculomotor capture. Percept Psychophys 67:910–928

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Briand KA, Larrison AL, Sereno AB (2000) Inhibition of return in manual and saccadic response systems Percept. Psychophysics 62:1512–1524

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Collins C, Barnes G (2006) The occluded onset pursuit paradigm: prolonging anticipatory smooth pursuit in the absence of visual feedback. Exp Brain Res 175:11–20

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dorris MC, Pare M, Munoz DP (1997) Neuronal activity in monkey superior colliculus related to the initiation of saccadic eye movements. J. Neuroscience 17:8566–8579

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dukelow SP, DeSouza JFX, Culham JC, van den Berg AV, Menon RS, Vilis T (2001) Distinguishing subregions of the human MT+ complex using visual fields and pursuit eye movements. J Neurophysiol 86:1991–2000

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Everling S, Dorris MC, Klein RM, Munoz DP (1999) Role of primate superior colliculus in preparation and execution of anti-saccades and pro-saccades. J Neurosci 19:2740–2754

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrera VP, Lisberger SG (1997) The effect of a moving distractor on the initiation of smooth pursuit eye movements. Vis Neurosci 14:323–338

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer B, Ramsperger E (1984) Human express saccades: extremely short reaction times of goal directed eye movements. Exp Brain Res 57:191–195

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer B, Weber H (1993) Express saccades and visual attention. Behav Brain Sci 16:553–610

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guimarães-Silva S, Gawryszewski LG, Portugal TS, Klausner-de-Oliveira L (2004) Inhibition of return, gap effect and saccadic reaction time to a visual target. Braz J Med Biol Res 37:533–538

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hallett P (1978) Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions. Vis Res 18:1279–1296

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Huk AC, Dougherty RF, Heeger DJ (2002) Retinotopy and functional subdivision of human areas MT and MST. J Neurosci 22:7195–7205

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kingstone A, Klein RM (1993) Visual offsets facilitate saccadic latency: does predisengagement of visuospatial attention mediate the gap effect? J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 19:1251–1265

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Klein RM (2000) Inhibition of return. Trends Cogn Sci 4:138–147

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knox PC (1996) The effect of the gap paradigm on the latency of human smooth pursuit eye movement. Neuroreport 7:3027–3030

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Knox PC (1998) Stimulus predictability and the gap effect on pre-saccadic smooth pursuit. Neuroreport 9:809–812

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Knox PC (2005) The gap effect on smooth pursuit investigated with long gaps, with and without fixation offsets. Soc Neurosci Abstr 475:11

    Google Scholar 

  • Knox PC, Bekour T (2002) Non-target influences on the initiation of smooth pursuit. Prog Brain Res 140:211–224

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knox PC, Bekour T (2004) Spatial mapping of the remote distractor effect on smooth pursuit initiation. Exp Brain Res 154:494–503

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Komatsu H, Wurtz RH (1988) Relation of cortical areas MT and MST to pursuit eye movements I. Localization and visual properties of neurons. J Neurophysiol 60:580–603

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kowler E (1989) Cognitive expectations not habits control anticipatory smooth ocular pursuit. Vis Res 29:1049–1057

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Krauzlis RJ (2003) Neuronal activity in the rostral superior colliculus related to the initiation of pursuit and saccadic eye movements. J Neurosci 23:4333–4344

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Krauzlis RJ (2004) Recasting the Smooth Pursuit Eye Movement System. J Neurophysiol 91:591–603

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krauzlis RJ, Miles FA (1996a) Decreases in the latency of smooth pursuit and saccadic eye movements produced by “ gap paradigm” in the monkey. Vis Res 36:1973–1985

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Krauzlis RJ, Miles FA (1996b) Release of fixation for pursuit and saccades in humans: evidence for shared inputs acting on different neural substrates. J Neurophysiol 76:2822–2833

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Krauzlis RJ, Basso MA, Wurtz RH (2000) Discharge properties of neurons in the rostral superior colliculus of the monkey during smooth pursuit eye movement. J Neurophysiol 84:876–891

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Leigh R, Zee D (1999) The neurology of eye movements. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy-Schoen A (1969) Determination et latence de la reponse oculomotrice a deux stimulus. L’Anee Psychol 74:43–66

    Google Scholar 

  • Maunsell JHR, van Essen DC (1983) Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey I. Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed and orientation. J Neurophysiol 49:1127–1147

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Missal M, Keller EL (2002) Common inhibitory mechanism for saccade and smooth pursuit eye movements. J Neurophysiol 88:1880–1892

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Munoz DP, Everling S (2004) Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement. Nat Rev Neurosci 5:218–228

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Munoz DP, Fecteau JH (2002) Vying for dominance: dynamic interactions control visual fixation and saccadic initiation in the superior colliculus. Prog Brain Res 140:3–19

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Mullane G, Knox PC (1999) Modification of smooth pursuit initiation by target contrast. Vis Res 39:3459–3464

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Orban de Xivry J-J, Lefevre P (2007) Saccades and pursuit: two outcomes of a single sensorimotor process. J Physiol 584:11–23

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pierrot-Deseilligny C, Muri RM, Ploner CJ, Gaymard B, Demeret S, Rivaud-Pechoux S (2003) Decisional role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in ocular motor behaviour. Brain 126:1460–1473

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rashbass C (1961) The relationship between saccadic and smooth tracking eye movements. J Physiol 159:326–338

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Reulen JPH (1984) Latency of visually evoked saccadic eye movements. Biol Cybern 50:251–262

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Reuter-Lorenz PA, Hughes HC, Fendrich R (1991) The reduction of saccadic latency by prior offset of the fixation point: an analysis of the gap effect Percept. Psychophysics 49:167–175

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rivaud S, Muri RM, Gaymard B, Vermersch AI, Pierrot-Deseilligny C (1994) Eye movement disorders after frontal eye field lesions in humans. Exp Brain Res 102:110–120

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ro T, Pratt J, Rafal RD (2000) Inhibition of return in saccadic eye movements. Exp Brain Res 130:264–268

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Samuel AG, Kat D (2003) Inhibition of return: a graphical meta-analysis of its time course and an empirical test of its temporal and spatial properties. Psych Bull Rev 10:897–906

    Google Scholar 

  • Saslow MG (1967) Effects of components of displacement step stimuli upon latency for saccadic eye movements. J Opt Soc Am 57:1024–1029

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sereno AB, Jeter CB, Pariyadath V, Briand KA (2006) Dissociating sensory and motor components of inhibition of return. Sci World 6:862–887

    Google Scholar 

  • Tanaka M, Lisberger SG (2000) Context-dependant smooth eye movements evoked by stationary visual stimuli in trained monkeys. J Neurophysiol 84:1748–1762

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Theeuwes J (1991) Exogenous and endogenous control of attention: The effect of visual onsets and offsets. Percept Psychophys 49:83–90

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Verghese P, Watamaniuk SNJ, McKee SP, Grzywacz NM (1999) Local motion detectors cannot account for the detectability of an extended trajectory in noise. Vis Res 39:19–30

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Walker R, Deubel H, Schneider WX, Findlay JM (1997) Effect of remote distractors on saccade programming: evidence for an extended fixation zone. J Neurophysiol 78:1108–1119

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paul C. Knox.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Knox, P.C. Prior information and oculomotor initiation: the effect of cues in gaps. Exp Brain Res 192, 75–85 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-008-1556-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-008-1556-9

Keywords

Navigation