Skip to main content
Log in

Effector- and target-independent representation of observed actions: evidence from incidental repetition priming

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Simulation mechanisms are thought to play an important role in action recognition. On this view, actions are represented through the re-enactment of the observed action. Mirror neurons are thought to be the neuronal counterpart of such a process, and code actions at a rather abstract level, often generalizing across sensory modalities and effectors. In humans, attention has been focussed on the somatotopic, effector dependent representation of observed actions in the mirror system. In this series of behavioural studies, we used incidental repetition priming to determine to which degree the cognitive representation of observed actions relies on effector- and target-dependent representations. Participants were presented with images depicting meaningless or meaningful actions and pressed a button only when presented with a meaningful action. Images were classified as depicting a repeated or new action, relative to the previous image in the trial series. In the first experiment, we demonstrate a priming effect based on the repetition of an action, performed by the same effector over the same target object. In the second experiment, we demonstrate that this facilitation holds even when the same action is performed over a different target object. Finally, in the third experiment we show that the action priming effect holds even when the same action is accomplished with different effectors. These results suggest the existence of a cognitive representation of actions, automatically activated during observation, which is abstract enough to generalize across different targets for that action and different effectors performing that action.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aziz-Zadeh L, Wilson SM, Rizzolatti G, Iacoboni M (2006) Congruent embodied representations for visually presented actions and linguistic phrases describing actions. Curr Biol 16:1818–1823

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou LW (1999) Perceptual symbol systems. Behav Brain Sci 22:577–609

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Binkofski F, Amunts K, Stephan K, Posse S, Schormann T, Freund H, Zilles K, Seitz R (2000) Broca’s region subserves imagery of motion: a combined cytoarchitectonic and fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp 11:273–285

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bekkering H, Wohlschlager A, Gattis M (2000) Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. Q J Exp Psychol A 53:153–164

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Borghi AM (2004) Object concepts and action: extracting affordances from objects parts. Acta Psychol 115:69–96

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brass M, Bekkering H, Prinz W (2001) Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. Acta Psychol 106:3–22

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Buccino G, Binkofski F, Fink GR, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Gallese V, Seitz RJ, Zilles K, Rizzolatti G, Freund HJ (2001) Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study. Eur J Neurosci 13:400–404

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buccino G, Riggio L, Melli G, Binkofski F, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G (2005) Listening to action-related sentences modulates the activity of the motor system: a combined TMS and behavioral study. Cogn Brain Res 24:355–363

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Buckner R, Koutstaal W (1998) Functional neuroimaging studies of encoding, priming, and explicit memory retrieval. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95:891–898

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Burstedt MKO, Benoni BE, Roland SJ (1997) Coordination of fingertip forces during human manipulation can emerge from independent neural networks controlling each engaged digit. Exp Brain Res 117:67–79

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Calinon S, Guenter F, Billard A (2005) Goal-directed imitation in a humanoid robot. In: IEEE international conference on robotics and automation, Barcelona, Spain

  • Catmur C, Walsh V, Heyes C (2007) Sensorimotor learning configures the human mirror system. Curr Biol 17:1527–1531

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coles M, Gratton G, Bashore T, Eriksen C, Donchin E (1985) A psychophysiological investigation of the continuous flow model of human information processing. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 11:529–553

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Costantini M, Galati G, Ferretti A, Caulo M, Tartaro A, Romani GL, Aglioti SM (2005) Neural systems underlying observation of humanly impossible movements: an fMRI study. Cereb Cortex 15:1761–1767

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Craighero L, Bello A, Fadiga L, Rizzolatti G (2002) Hand action preparation influences the responses to hand pictures. Neuropsychologia 40:492–502

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cross ES, AFdC Hamilton, Grafton ST (2006) Building a motor simulation de novo: observation of dance by dancers. Neuroimage 31:1257–1267

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene S, Naccache L, Clec’H GL, Koechlin E, Mueller M, Dehaene-Lambertz G, van de Moortele PF, Le Bihan D (1998) Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature 395:597–600

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Jong R, Liang C, Lauber E (1994) Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus–response correspondence. J Exp Psychol Human Percept Perform 20:731–750

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Di Pellegrino G, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G (1992) Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study. Exp Brain Res 91:176–180

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Di Pellegrino G, Rafal R, Tipper SP (2005) Implicitly evoked actions modulate visual selection: evidence from parietal extinction. Curr Biol 15:1469–1472

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Pavesi G, Rizzolatti G (1995) Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. J Neurophysiol 73:2608–2611

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fogassi L, Gallese V, Fadiga L, Rizzolatti G (1998) Neurons responding to the sight of goal-directed hand/arm actions in the parietal area PF (7b) of the macaque monkey. Soc. Neurosci. Abs. 275.5

  • Fogassi L, Ferrari PF, Gesierich B, Rozzi S, Chersi F, Rizzolatti G (2005) Parietal lobe: from action organization to intention understanding. Science 308:662–667

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Galati G, Committeri G, Aprile T, Spitoni G, Di Russo F, Pitzalis S, Pizzamiglio L (2008) A selective representation of the meaning of actions in the auditory mirror system. Neuroimage 40:1274–1286. doi:doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.044

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher S (2007) Simulation trouble. Soc Neurosci 2:353–365

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese V, Lakoff G (2005) The brain’s concepts: the role of the sensory-motor system in reason and language. Cogn Neuropsychol 22:455–479

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese V, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Rizzolatti G (1996) Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain 119:593–609

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gazzola V, Aziz-Zadeh L, Keysers C (2006) Empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in humans. Curr Biol 16:1824–1829

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gazzola V, van der Worp H, Mulder T, Wicker B, Rizzolatti G, Keysers C (2007) Aplasics born without hands mirror the goal of hand actions with their feet. Curr Biol 17:1235–1240

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson JJ (1979) The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton-Mifflin, Boston

    Google Scholar 

  • Grafton ST, Fadiga L, Arbib MA, Rizzolatti G (1997) Premotor cortex activation during observation and naming of familiar tools. Neuroimage 6:231–236

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grezes J, Decety J (2002) Does visual perception of object afford action? Evidence from a neuroimaging study. Neuropsychologia 40:212–222

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grezes J, Armony JL, Rowe J, Passingham RE (2003) Activations related to “mirror” and “canonical” neurones in the human brain: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 18:928–937

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton AF, Grafton ST (2006) Goal representation in human anterior intraparietal sulcus. J Neurosci 26:1133–1137

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton AF, Grafton ST (2007) The motor hierarchy: from kinematics to goals and intentions. In: Haggard P, Rossetti Y, Kawato M (eds) Sensorimotor foundations of higher cognition. Attention and performance XXII. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Hanson C, Hirst W (1989) On the representation of events: a study of orientation, recall, and recognition. J Exp Psychol Gen 118:136–147

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hauk O, Johnsrude I, Pulvermüller F (2004) Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron 41:301–307

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henson RNA, Rugg MD (2003) Neural response suppression, haemodynamic repetition effects, and behavioural priming. Neuropsychologia 41:263–270

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes O, Abbs JH (1976) Labial–mandibular coordination in the production of speech: Implications for the operation of motor equivalence. Phonetica 44:199–221

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni M, Woods RP, Brass M, Bekkering H, Mazziotta JC, Rizzolatti G (1999) Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science 286:2526–2528

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni M, Molnar-Szakacs I, Gallese V, Buccino G, Mazziotta J, Rizzolatti G (2005) Grasping the intentions of others with one’s own mirror neuron system. PLoS Biol 3:e79

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kable JW, Chatterjee A (2006) Specificity of action representations in the lateral occipitotemporal cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 18:1498–1517

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kable JW, Lease-Spellmeyer J, Chatterjee A (2002) Neural substrates of action event knowledge. J Cogn Neurosci 14:795–805

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kable JW, Kan IP, Wilson A, Thompson-Schill SL, Chatterjee A (2005) Conceptual representations of action in the lateral temporal cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 17:1855–1870

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keele SW, Cohen A, Ivry R (1990) Motor programs: concepts and issues. In: Jeannerod M (ed) Attention and performance, XIII: motor representation and control. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 77–107

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohler E, Keysers C, Umiltà MA, Fogassi L, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G (2002) Hearing sounds, understanding actions: action representation in mirror neurons. Science 297:846–848

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koski L, Wohlschlager A, Bekkering H, Woods RP, Dubeau M-C, Mazziotta JC, Iacoboni M (2002) Modulation of motor and premotor activity during imitation of target-directed actions. Cereb Cortex 12:847–855

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kourtzi Z, Kanwisher N (2000) Cortical regions involved in perceiving object shape. J Neurosci 20:3310–3318

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lahav A, Saltzman E, Schlaug G (2007) Action representation of sound: audiomotor recognition network while listening to newly acquired actions. J Neurosci 27:308–314

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lashley K (1951) The problem of serial order in behavior. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Majdandzic J, Grol MJ, van Schie HT, Verhagen L, Toni I, Bekkering H (2007) The role of immediate and final goals in action planning: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 37:589

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller GA, Galanter E, Pribram K (1960) Plans and the structure of behavior. Rinehart and Winston, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Murata A, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Gallese V, Raos V, Rizzolatti G (1997) Object representation in the ventral premotor cortex (area F5) of the monkey. J Neurophysiol 78:2226–2230

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Newman-Norlund RD, van Schie HT, van Zuijlen AMJ, Bekkering H (2007) The mirror neuron system is more active during complementary compared with imitative action. Nat Neurosci 10:817–818

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Newtson D (1973) Attribution and the unit of perception of ongoing behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol 28:28–38

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips JC, Ward R (2002) S-R correspondence effects of irrelevant visual affordance: time course and specificity of response activation. Vis Cogn 9:540–548

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pizzamiglio L, Aprile T, Spitoni G, Pitzalis S, Bates E, D’Amico S, Di Russo F (2005) Separate neural systems for processing action- or non-action-related sounds. Neuroimage 24:852–861

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pobric G, de C Hamilton AF (2006) Action understanding requires the left inferior frontal cortex. Curr Biol 16:524–529

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schacter DL, Buckner RL (1998) Priming and the brain. Neuron 20:185–195

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tettamanti M, Buccino G, Saccuman MC, Gallese V, Danna M, Scifo P, Fazio F, Rizzolatti G, Cappa SF, Perani D (2005) Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits. J Cogn Neurosci 17:273–281

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tucker M, Ellis R (1998) On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 24:830–846

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky B, Hemenway K (1984) Objects, parts, and categories. J Exp Psychol Gen 113:169–193

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Umiltà MA, Kohler E, Gallese V, Fogassi L, Fadiga L, Keysers C, Rizzolatti G (2001) I know what you are doing: a neurophysiological study. Neuron 31:155–165

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Umiltà MA, Escola L, Intskirveli I, Grammont F, Rochat M, Caruana F, Jezzini A, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G (2008) When pliers become fingers in the monkey motor system. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105:2209–2213. doi:10.1073/pnas.0705985105

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Urgesi C, Candidi M, Ionta S, Aglioti SM (2007) Representation of body identity and body actions in extrastriate body area and ventral premotor cortex. Nat Neurosci 10:30–31

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vallacher RR, Wegner DM (1987) What do people think they’re doing? Action identification and human behavior. Psychol Rev 94:3–15

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Francesca Defeudis and Antonello Baldassarre for their help in collecting and analyzing data, and to Bernhard Hommel for his comments on a previous version of this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marcello Costantini.

Electronic supplementary material

This material is unfortunately not in the Publisher's archive anymore:

Meaningful actions used in experiments 1 and 3. (TIFF 1341 kb)

Meaningless actions used in all the experiments. (TIFF 603 kb)

Meaningful actions used in experiment 2. (TIFF 1347 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Costantini, M., Committeri, G. & Galati, G. Effector- and target-independent representation of observed actions: evidence from incidental repetition priming. Exp Brain Res 188, 341–351 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-008-1369-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-008-1369-x

Keywords

Navigation