Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 180, Issue 4, pp 727–736 | Cite as

Perceptual learning: how much daily training is enough?

Research Article

Abstract

The acquisition of many perceptual skills proceeds over a course of days. However, little is known about how much daily training is needed for such learning to occur. Here we investigated this question by examining how varying the number of training trials per day affected learning over multiple days on two auditory discrimination tasks: frequency discrimination and temporal-interval discrimination. For each task, we compared improvements in discrimination thresholds between different groups of listeners who were trained for either 360 or 900 trials per day for 6 days. Improvement on frequency discrimination required >360 trials of training per day while learning on temporal-interval discrimination occurred with 360 training trials per day, and additional daily practice did not increase the amount of improvement. It therefore appears that the accumulation of improvement over days on auditory discrimination tasks may require some critical amount of training per day, that training beyond that critical amount yields no additional learning on the trained condition, and that the critical amount of training needed varies across tasks. These results imply that perceptual skills are transferred from short- to long-term memory (consolidated) daily, but only if a task-specific initiation requirement has been met.

References

  1. Ahissar M, Hochstein S (1997) Task difficulty and the specificity of perceptual learning. Nature 387:401–406PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Brashers-Krug T, Shadmehr R, Bizzi E (1996) Consolidation in human motor memory. Nature 382:252–255PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Brechmann A, Scheich H (2005) Hemispheric shifts of sound representation in auditory cortex with conceptual listening. Cereb Cortex 15:578–587PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Caithness G, Osu R, Bays P, Chase H, Klassen J, Kawato M, Wolpert DM, Flanagan JR (2004) Failure to consolidate the consolidation theory of learning for sensorimotor adaptation tasks. J Neurosci 24:8662–8671PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Campbell RA, Small AM (1963) Effect of practice and feedback on frequency discrimination. J Acoust Soc Am 35:1511–1514CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Cohen J (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. In: The concepts of power analysis. Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc, HillsdaleGoogle Scholar
  7. Delhommeau K, Micheyl C, Jouvent R, Collet L (2002) Transfer of learning across durations and ears in auditory frequency discrimination. Percept Psychophys 64:426–436PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Demany L (1985) Perceptual learning in frequency discrimination. J Acoust Soc Am 78:1118–1120PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Grimault N, Micheyl C, Carlyon RP, Bacon SP, Collet L (2003) Learning in discrimination of frequency or modulation rate: generalization to fundamental frequency discrimination. Hear Res 184:41–50PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Hauptmann B, Karni A (2002) From primed to learn: the saturation of repetition priming and the induction of long-term memory. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 13:313–322PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Hauptmann B, Reinhart E, Brandt SA, Karni A (2005) The predictive value of the leveling off of within session performance for procedural memory consolidation. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 24:181–189PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Irvine DR, Martin RL, Klimkeit E, Smith R (2000) Specificity of perceptual learning in a frequency discrimination task. J Acoust Soc Am 108:2964–2968PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Karmarkar UR, Buonomano DV (2003) Temporal specificity of perceptual learning in an auditory discrimination task. Learn Mem 10:141–147PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Karni A, Sagi D (1993) The time course of learning a visual skill. Nature 365:250–252PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Krakauer JW, Ghilardi MF, Ghez C (1999) Independent learning of internal models for kinematic and dynamic control of reaching. Nat Neurosci 2:1026–1031PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Levitt H (1971) Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. J Acoust Soc Am 49:467–477PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. McGaugh JL (2000) Memory–a century of consolidation. Science 287:248–251PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Meegan DV, Aslin RN, Jacobs RA (2000) Motor timing learned without motor training. Nat Neurosci 3:860–862PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Nagarajan SS, Blake DT, Wright BA, Byl N, Merzenich MM (1998) Practice-related improvements in somatosensory interval discrimination are temporally specific but generalize across skin location, hemisphere, and modality. J Neurosci 18:1559–1570PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Nenadic I, Gaser C, Volz HP, Rammsayer T, Hager F, Sauer H (2003) Processing of temporal information and the basal ganglia: new evidence from fMRI. Exp Brain Res 148:238–246PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Ofen-Noy N, Dudai Y, Karni A (2003) Skill learning in mirror reading: how repetition determines acquisition. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 17:507–521PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Roth DA-E, Kishon-Rabin L, Hildesheimer M, Karni A, Roth DA-Erini, Roth DA (2005) A latent consolidation phase in auditory identification learning: time in the awake state is sufficient. Learn Memory 12:159–164Google Scholar
  23. Savion-Lemieux T, Penhune VB (2005) The effects of practice and delay on motor skill learning and retention. Exp Brain Res 161:423–431PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Seitz AR, Yamagishi N, Werner B, Goda N, Kawato M, Watanabe T (2005) Task-specific disruption of perceptual learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:14895–14900PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Shadmehr R, Brashers-Krug T (1997) Functional stages in the formation of human long-term motor memory. J Neurosci 17:409–419PubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Walker MP, Brakefield T, Hobson JA, Stickgold R (2003) Dissociable stages of human memory consolidation and reconsolidation. Nature 425:616–620PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Wright BA (1998) Generalization of auditory-discrimination learning. In: Association for research in otolaryngology abstracts, p 413Google Scholar
  28. Wright BA, Fitzgerald MB (2005) Learning and generalization on five basic auditory discrimination tasks as assessed by threshold changes. In: Pressnitzer D, de Cheveigne A, McAdams S, Collet L (eds) Auditory signal processing: physiology, psychoacoustics, and models. Springer, New York, pp 510–516Google Scholar
  29. Wright BA, Buonomano DV, Mahncke HW, Merzenich MM (1997) Learning and generalization of auditory temporal-interval discrimination in humans. J Neurosci 17:3956–3963PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Zatorre RJ, Belin P, Penhune VB (2002) Structure and function of auditory cortex: music and speech. Trends Cogn Sci 6:37–46PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Communication Sciences and DisordersNorthwestern UniversityEvanstonUSA

Personalised recommendations