Skip to main content
Log in

Perception of non-voluntary brief contractions in normal subjects and in a deafferented patient

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The accuracy of force perception by human subjects in the absence of voluntary motor command was evaluated by exploring how they perceived isometric twitches of wrist extensor muscles produced by external stimulation. Twelve normal subjects and a well-known patient lacking large-diameter afferent fibres (GL) performed estimation, production and reproduction tasks. Magnetic stimulation of the radial nerve and, in normal subjects, transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) of the motor cortex were used to produce weak brief non-voluntary twitches. In estimation tasks, the subjects had to ascribe verbal marks on a 1–5 scale to the forces of stimulation-induced twitches. Loose covariations of marks and forces were observed, while directions of force variations between successive twitches were relatively well detected. GL did less well than normal subjects in detecting directions of force variations. In production tasks, subjects had to produce twitches matching verbal command marks in a 1–5 range, with or without visual feedback. Performances of normal subjects and GL resembled those of estimation tasks and were not improved by visual feedback. In reproduction tasks, subjects had to duplicate stimulation-induced test twitches: first without visual feedback, second with and third again without. Large errors were observed but all subjects did better with visual feedback. In the third step, improvement with respect to the first one was significantly more marked with TMS than with peripheral stimulation. GL improved her performance in the third step, possibly because she could use information provided by group III and group IV afferents still present in her nerves. Altogether, for normal subjects (1) the performances in estimation tasks are consistent with the known behaviour of Golgi tendon organs as observed in animal experiments, and (2) results observed in reproduction tasks suggest that cortical stimulation might elicit, in addition to corticospinal activation of motoneurones, collateral discharges that could be stored as a memory of motor command.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1 a
Fig. 2 a
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5 a
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Amassian VH, Cracco RQ, Maccabee PJ (1989) A sense of movement elicited in paralyzed distal arm by focal magnetic coil stimulation of human motor cortex. Brain Res 479:355–360

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Angel RW (1980) Barognosis in a patient with hemiataxia. Ann Neurol 7:73–77

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Banister EW (1979) The perception of effort: an inductive approach. Eur J Appl Physiol 41:141–150

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bawa P, Lemon RN (1993) Recruitment of motor units in response to transcranial magnetic stimulation in man. J Physiol (Lond) 471:445–464

    Google Scholar 

  • Bessou P, Laporte Y (1960) Activation des fibres afférentes myélinisées de petit calibre d’origine musculaire (fibres du groupe III). C R Soc Biol 154:1093–1096

    Google Scholar 

  • Brasil-Neto JP, Valls-Sole J, Pascual-Leone A, Cammarota A, Amasian VE, Cracco R, Maccabee P, Cracco J, Hallett M, Cohen LG (1993) Rapid modulation of human motor output following ischaemic nerve block. Brain 116:511–525

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brodie EE, Ross HE (1984) Sensorimotor mechanisms in weight discrimination. Percept Psychophys 36:477–481

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burgess PR, Jones LF (1997) Perception of effort and heaviness during fatigue and during the size-weight illusion. Somatosens Mot Res 14:189–202

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burke D (1981) The activity of human muscle spindle endings in normal motor behavior. In: Porter R (ed) International review of physiology, vol 25. University Park Press, Baltimore, pp 91–126

  • Cafarelli E, Bigland-Ritchie B (1979) Sensation of static force in muscles of different lengths. Exp Neurol 65:511–525

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cole JD, Merton WL, Barett G, Katifi HA, Treede RD (1995) Evoked potentials in a subject with a large-fibre sensory neuropathy below the neck. Can J Physiol Pharmacol 75:234–245

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole JD, Sedgwick EM (1992) The perception of force and of movement in a man without large myelinated sensory afferents below the neck. J Physiol (Lond) 449:503–515

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper DF, Grimby G, Jones DA, Edwards RHT (1979) Perception of effort in isometric and dynamic muscular contraction. Eur J Appl Physiol 41:173–180

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Eddin BB, Abbs JH (1991) Finger movement responses of cutaneous mechanoreceptors in the dorsal skin of the human hand. J Neurophysiol 65:657–670

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ellaway PH, Murphy PR, Tripathi A (1982) Closely coupled excitation of gamma motoneurones by group III muscle afferents with low mechanical threshold in the cat. J Physiol (Lond) 331:481–498

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellaway PH, Prochazka A, Chan M, Gauthier MJ (2004)The sense of movement ellicited by transcranial magnetic stimulation in humans is due to sensory feedback. J. Physiol (Lond) 556:651–660

    Google Scholar 

  • Fleury M, Bard C, Teasdale N, Paillard J, Cole J, Lajoie Y, Lamarre Y (1995) Weight judgment. The discrimination capacity of a deafferented subject. Brain 118:1149–1156

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Forget R, Lamarre Y (1995) Motor programming in the absence of sensory afferents in man. Can J Physiol Pharmacol 73:285–294

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fukami Y, Wilkinson RS (1979) Responses of isolated tendon organs of the cat. J Physiol (Lond) 265:673–689

    Google Scholar 

  • Gandevia SC (1996) Kinesthesia: roles for afferent signals and motor commands. In: Handbook of physiology, exercise regulation and integration of multiple systems, Sect 12, Chap 4. American Physiological Society, Bethesda, MD, pp 128–172

  • Gandevia SC, Kilbreath SL (1990) Accuracy of weight estimation for weights lifted by proximal and distal muscles of the human upper limb. J Physiol (Lond) 423:299–310

    Google Scholar 

  • Gooch JL, Randle J (1993) Force perception before and after maximal voluntary contraction. Percept Mot Skills 76:399–402

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon AM, Westling G, Cole KJ, Johansson RS (1993) Memory representations underlying motor commands used during manipulation of common and novel objects. J Neurophysiol 69:1789–1796

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gregory JE, Proske U (1979) The responses of Golgi tendon organs to stimulation of different combinations of motor units. J Physiol (Lond) 295:251–262

    Google Scholar 

  • Hager-Ross C, Johansson RS (1996) Non-digital afferent input in reactive control of fingertip forces during precision grip. Exp Brain Res 110:131–141

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton A, Wolpert D, Frith U (2004) Your own action influences how you perceive another person’s action. Curr Biol 14:493–498

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henningsen H, Ende-Henningsen B, Gordon AW (1995) Contribution of tactile afferent information to the control of isometric finger force. Exp Brain Res 105:312–317

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henningsen H, Knecht S, Ende-Henningsen B (1997) Influence of afferent feedback on isometric fine force resolution in humans. Exp Brain Res 113:207–213

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horcholle-Bossavit G, Jami L, Petit J, Vejsada R, Zytnicki D (1990) Ensemble discharge from Golgi tendon organs of the cat peroneus tertius muscle. J Neurophysiol 64:813–821

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Houk JC, Henneman E (1967) Responses of Golgi tendon organs to active contraction of the soleus muscle of the cat. J Neurophysiol 30:466–481

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson AW, Dishman K (2000) Perceived submaximal force production in young adult males and females. Med Sci Sport Exerc 32:448–451

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jami L (1992) Golgi tendon organs in mammalian skeletal muscle: functional properties and central actions. Physiol Rev 72:623–666

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones LA (1989) Matching forces: constant errors and differential thresholds. Perception 18:681–687

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones LA (1994) Peripheral mechanisms of touch and proprioception. Can J Physiol Pharmacol 72:484–487

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones LA (2000) Visual and haptic feedback in the control of force. Exp Brain Res 130:269–272

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg A (1999) Descending control of forelimb movements in the cat.Brain Res Bull 50:323–324

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCloskey DI, Ebeling P, Goodwin GM (1974) Estimation of weights and tensions and apparent involvement of a “sense of effort”. Exp Neurol 42:220–232

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCloskey DI, Prochazka A (1994) The role of sensory information in the guidance of voluntary movement: reflections on a symposium held at the 22nd Annual Meeting of the Society for Neuroscience. Somatosens Mot Res 11:69–76

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McIntyre AK, Proske U, Rawson JA (1984) Cortical projection of afferent information from tendon organs in the cat. J Physiol (Lond) 354:395–406

    Google Scholar 

  • Mai N, Avarello M, Bolsinger P (1985) Maintenance of low isometric forces during prehensile grasping. Neuropsychology 23:805–812

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mai N, Schreiber P, Hermsdorfer J (1991) Changes in perceived finger force produced by muscular contractions under isometric and anisometric conditions. Exp Brain Res 84:453–460

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mense S, Meyer H (1985) Different types of slowly conducting afferent units in cat skeletal muscle and tendon. J Physiol (Lond) 363:403–417

    Google Scholar 

  • Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychology 9:97–113

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Paintal AS (1960) Functional analysis of group III afferent fibres of mammalian muscle. J Physiol (Lond) 152:250–270

    Google Scholar 

  • Pierrot-Deseilligny E (2002) Propriospinal transmission of part of the corticospinal excitation in humans. Muscle Nerve 26:155–172

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roland PE, Ladegaard-Petersen H (1977) A quantitative analysis of sensations of tension and of kinaesthesia in man. Evidence for a peripherally originating muscular sense and for a sense of effort. Brain 100:671–692

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rossi A, Mazzocchio R, Parlanti S (1991) Cortical projection of putative group Ib afferent fibres from the human forearm. Brain Res 547:62–68

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rothwell JC (1997) Techniques and mechanisms of action of transcranial stimulation of the human motor cortex. J Neurosci Methods 74:113–122

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rothwell JC, Traub MM, Day BL, Obeso JA, Thomas PK, Marsden CD (1982) Manual motor performance in a deafferented man. Brain 105:515–542

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sanes JN, Shadmehr R (1995) Sense of muscular effort and somesthetic afferent information in humans. Can J Physiol Pharmacol 73:223–233

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens SS (1975) Psychophysics. Wiley, New York

  • Stuart A, Ord JK (1991) Kendall’s advanced theory of statistics, 5th edn. Edward Arnold, London

  • Teasdale N, Forget R, Bard C, Paillard J, Fleury M, Lamarre Y (1993) The role of proprioceptive information for production of isometric forces and for handwriting tasks. Acta Psychol (Amst) 82:179–191

    Google Scholar 

  • Vallbo AB (1995) Single-afferent neurons and somatic sensations in humans. In: Gazzaniga MS (ed) The cognitive neurosciences. MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass., pp 237–252

  • Waller AD (1891) The sense of effort: an objective study. Brain 14:179–249

    Google Scholar 

  • Zytnicki D, Lafleur J, Kouchtir N, Perrier JF (1995) Heterogeneity of contraction-induced effects in neurons of the cat dorsal spino-cerebellar tract. J Physiol (Lond) 487:761–772

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank Pr. Y. Laporte, Pr. Y. Galifret and Dr. D. Zytnicki for stimulating discussions and helpful suggestions, and Dr. L. Mazieres for judicious comments on the manuscript. Our thanks also go to Pr. J. Paillard for kindly attending the experiments with GL.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to L. Jami.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Nicolas, G., Marchand-Pauvert, V., Lasserre, V. et al. Perception of non-voluntary brief contractions in normal subjects and in a deafferented patient. Exp Brain Res 161, 166–179 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-004-2056-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-004-2056-1

Keywords

Navigation