Skip to main content
Log in

Amplitude and direction errors in kinesthetic pointing

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We investigated the accuracy with which, in the absence of vision, one can reach again a 2D target location that had been previously identified by a guided movement. A robotic arm guided the participant’s hand to a target (locating motion) and away from it (homing motion). Then, the participant pointed freely toward the remembered target position. Two experiments manipulated separately the kinematics of the locating and homing motions. Some robot motions followed a straight path with the bell-shaped velocity profile that is typical of natural movements. Other motions followed curved paths, or had strong acceleration and deceleration peaks. Current motor theories of perception suggest that pointing should be more accurate when the homing and locating motion mimics natural movements. This expectation was not borne out by the results, because amplitude and direction errors were almost independent of the kinematics of the locating and homing phases. In both experiments, participants tended to overshoot the target positions along the lateral directions. In addition, pointing movements towards oblique targets were attracted by the closest diagonal (oblique effect). This error pattern was robust not only with respect to the manner in which participants located the target position (perceptual equivalence), but also with respect to the manner in which they executed the pointing movements (motor equivalence). Because of the similarity of the results with those of previous studies on visual pointing, it is argued that the observed error pattern is basically determined by the idiosyncratic properties of the mechanisms whereby space is represented internally.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1A–C
Fig. 2A–D
Fig. 3
Fig. 4A–C
Fig. 5A–D
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9
Fig. 10A–F
Fig. 11A–E

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adamovich AV, Berkinblit MB, Fookson O, Poizner H (1998) Pointing in 3D space to remembered targets. I. Kinesthetic versus visual target presentation. J Neurophysiol 79:2833–2846

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adamovich AV, Berkinblit MB, Fookson O, Poizner H (1999) Pointing in 3D space to remembered targets. II. Effect of movement speed toward kinesthetically defined targets. Exp Brain Res 125:200–210

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Appelle S (1972) Percept and discrimination as a function of stimulus orientation: the “oblique effect” in man and animals. Psychol Bull 78:266–278

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Appelle S, Countryman M (1986) Eliminating the haptic oblique effect: influence of scanning incongruity and prior knowledge of the standards. Perception 15:365–369

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkeson CG, Hollerbach JM (1985) Kinematic features of unrestrained vertical arm movements. J Neurosci 5:2318–2330

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baud-Bovy G, Viviani P (1998) Pointing to kinesthetic targets in space. J Neurosci 18:1528–1545

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beardsworth T, Buckner T (1981) The ability to recognize oneself from a video recording of one’s movements without seeing one’s body. Bull Psychonom Soc 18:19–22

    Google Scholar 

  • Bookstein (1979) Fitting conic section to scattered data. Comput Graph Image Pro 9:56–71

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheng MFH (1968) Tactile-kinesthetic percept of length. Am J Psychol 81:74–82

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crowder MJ, Hand DJ (1995) Analysis of repeated measures. Monographs on statistics and applied probability 41, Chapman & Hall, London

  • Cruse H, Brüwer M (1987) The human arm as a redundant manipulator: the control of path and joint angles. Biol Cybern 57:137–144

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • D’Amico M, Ferrigno G (1990) Technique for the evaluation of derivatives from noisy biomechanical data using a model-based bandwidth selection procedure. Med Biol Eng Comput 28:407–415

    Google Scholar 

  • Darling WG, Miller GF (1993) Transformations between visual and kinesthetic coordinate systems in reaches to remembered object locations and orientations. Exp Brain Res 93:534–547

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davidon RS, Cheng MFH (1964) Apparent distance in a horizontal plane with tactile-kinesthetic stimuli. Q J Exp Psychol 16:277–281

    Google Scholar 

  • Day RH, Wong TS (1971) Radial and tangential movement directions as determinants of the haptic illusion in an L figure. J Exp Psychol 87:19–22

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deregowski J, Ellis D (1972) Effect of stimulus orientation upon haptic Percept of the horizontal-vertical illusion. J Exp Psychol 95:14–19

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Desmurget M, Jordan M, Prablanc C, Jeannerod M (1997) Constrained and unconstrained movements involve different control strategies. J Neurophysiol 77:1644–1650

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de’Sperati C, Viviani P (1997) The relationship between curvature and velocity in two-dimensional smooth pursuit eye movements. J Neurosci 17:3932–3945

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dick M, Hochstein S (1989) Visual orientation estimation. Percept Psychophys 46:227–234

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Essock EA (1980) The oblique effect of stimulus identification considered with respect to two classes of oblique effects. Perception 9:37–46

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Essock EA, Krebs WK, Prather JR (1997) Superior sensitivity for tactile stimulus oriented proximally-distally on the finger: implication for mixed Class 1 and Class 2 anisotropies. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 23:515–527

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Faineteau H, Gentaz E, Viviani P (2003) The kinesthetic perception of Euclidian distance: a study of the detour effect. Exp Brain Res 152:166–172

    Google Scholar 

  • Flash T (1987) The control of hand equilibrium trajectories in multi-joint arm movements. Biol Cybern 57:257–274

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gander W, Golub GH, Strebel R (1994) Fitting circles and ellipses—least-squares solutions. Technical Report No. 217, Department of Computer Science, Swiss Federal Institute of Technology of Zürich (ETHZ), Switzerland

  • Gentaz E, Hatwell Y (1995) The haptic ‘oblique effect’ in children’s and adults’ Percept of orientation. Perception 24:631–646

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gentaz E, Hatwell Y (1996) Role of gravitational cues in the haptic Percept of orientation. Percept Psychophys 58:1278–1292

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ghez C, Gordon J, Ghilardi MF, Christatkos CN, Cooper SE (1990) Role of proprioceptive input in the programming of arm trajectories. Cold Spring Harbor Symposium of Quantitative Biology 55:837–847

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon J, Ghilardi MF, Ghez C (1994a) Accuracy of planar reaching movements. I. Independence of direction and extent variability. Exp Brain Res 99:97–111

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon J, Ghilardi MF, Cooper SE, Ghez C (1994b) Accuracy of planar reaching movements. II. Systematic extent errors resulting from inertial anisotropy. Exp Brain Res 99:112–130

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon J, Ghilardi MF, Ghez C (1995) Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. I. Spatial errors. J Neurophysiol 73:347–360

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gourtzelidis P, Smyrnis N, Evdokimidis I, Balogh A (2001) Systematic errors of planar movements provide evidence for space categorization effects and interaction of multiple frames of reference. Exp Brain Res 139:59–69

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haggard P, Richardson J (1996) Spatial patterns in the control of human arm movement. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 22:42–62

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Helms-Tillery SI, Flanders M, Soechting JF (1994) Errors in kinesthetic transformations for hand apposition. Neuroreport 6:177–181

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Howard IP (1982) Human visual orientation. Wiley, Chichester

  • Huttenlocher J, Hedges LV, Duncan S (1991) Categories and particulars: prototype effects in estimating spatial location. Psychol Rev 98:352–376

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Imanaka K, Abernethy B (1990) Interference between movement location and distance cues in the reproduction of slow and fast movements. J Hum Mov Stud 18:251–268

    Google Scholar 

  • Imanaka K, Abernethy B (2002) Distance-location interference in movement reproduction. In: Rossetti Y, Revonsuo A (eds) Behond dissociation: interaction between dissociated implicit and explicit processing. Advances in consciousness research. John Benjamins Publishing Co., Amsterdam, pp 41–71

  • Johansson G (1977) Studies on visual perception of locomotion. Perception 6:365–376

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kandel S, Orliaguet JP, Viviani P (2000) Perceptual anticipation in handwriting: the role of implicit motor competence. Percept Psychophys 62:706–716

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Karst GM, Hasan Z (1991a) Initiation rules for planar, two-joint arm movements: agonist selection for movement throughout the work space. J Neurophysiol 66:1579–1593

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lacquaniti F, Terzuolo C, Viviani P (1983) The law relating the kinematic and figural aspects of drawing movements. Acta Psychol 54:115–130

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Laszlo JI (1992) Motor control and learning: how far do the experimental tasks restrict our theoretical insight? In: Summers JJ (ed) Approaches to the study of motor control and learning. Elsevier, Amsterdam

  • Lechelt EC, Verenka A (1980) Spatial anisotropy in intramodal and cross-modal judgements of stimulus orientations: the stability of the oblique effect. Perception 9:581–589

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lederman SJ, Klatzky RL, Barber P (1985) Spatial- and movement-based heuristics for encoding pattern information through touch. J Exp Psychol Gen 114:33–49

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman AM, Mattingly IG (1985) The motor theory of speech Percept revised. Cognition 21:1–36

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McIntyre J, Stratta F, Lacquaniti F (1997) A viewer-center frame of reference for pointing to memorized targets in three dimensional space. J Neurophysiol 78:1601–1618

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McIntyre J, Stratta F, Lacquaniti F (1998) Short-term memory for reaching to visual targets: psychophysical evidence for body-centered reference frames. J Neurosci 18:8423–8435

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McIntyre J, Stratta F, Droulez J, Lacquaniti F (2000) Analysis of pointing errors reveals properties of data representations and coordinate transformations within the central nervous system. Neural Comput 12:2823–2855

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Messier J, Kalaska JF (1999) Comparison of variability of initial kinematics and endpoints of reaching movements. Exp Brain Res 125:139–152

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Muller PE, Sidorsky RC, Slivinske AJ, Alluis EA, Fitts PM (1955) The symbolic coding of information on cathode ray tubes and similar display. U.S.A.F. WADC. Technical Report No. 55–375

  • Poulton EC (1975) Range effects in experiments on people. Am J Psychol 88:3–33

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz W (1997) Percept and action planning. Eur J Cogn Psychol 9:129–154

    Google Scholar 

  • Rabiner LR, Gold B (1975) Theory and application of digital signal processing. Prentice-Hall, Englewoods Cliffs, NJ

  • Rao AK, Gordon AM (2001) Contribution of tactile information to accuracy in pointing movements. Exp Brain Res 138:438–445

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Reese EP (1953) Summary report No. SDC 131–1-5. Psychophysiological Research Unit, Mount Holyoke College, Massachusetts

  • Reid RL (1954) An illusion of movement complementarity to the horizontal-vertical illusion. Q J Exp Psychol 9:107–111

    Google Scholar 

  • Rossetti Y, Régnier C (1995) Representations in action: pointing to a target with various representations. In: Brady BG, Bootsma RJ, Guiard Y (eds) Studies in perception and action III. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 233–236

  • Rossetti Y, Gaunet F, Thinus-Blanc C (1996) Early visual experiment affects memorization and spatial representation of proprioceptive targets. Neuroreport 7:1219–1223

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sainburg RL, Ghilardi MF, Poizner H, Ghez C (1995) Control of limb dynamics in normal participants and patients without proprioception. J Neurophysiol 73:820–835

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smyrnis N, Gourtzelidis P, Evdokimidis I (2000) A systematic directional error in 2D arm movements increases with increasing delay between visual target presentation and movement execution. Exp Brain Res 131:111–120

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smyth MM (1984) Memory for movements. In: Smyth MM, Wing A (eds) The psychology of human movement. Academic Press, New York, pp 83–117

  • Soechting JF (1982) Does the sense at the elbow reflect a sense of elbow joint or one of limb orientation? Brain Res 248:392–395

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Soechting JF, Flanders M (1992) Moving in three dimensional space: frames of reference, vectors, and coordinate systems. Ann Rev Neurosci 15:167–191

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vindras P, Viviani P (1998) Frames of reference and control parameters in visuomanual pointing. J Exp Psychol Hum 24:569–591

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Viviani P, Flash T (1995) Minimum-jerk, two-third power law, and isochrony: converging approaches to movement planning. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 21:32–53

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Viviani P, Mounoud P (1990) Perceptuo-motor compatibility in pursuit tracking of two-dimensional movements. J Mot Behav 22:407–443

    Google Scholar 

  • Viviani P, Schneider R (1991) A developmental study of the relationship between geometry and kinematics in drawing movements. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 17:198–218

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Viviani P, Stucchi N (1992) Biological movements look uniform. Evidence of motor-perceptual interactions. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 86:603–623

    Google Scholar 

  • Viviani P, Mounoud P, Campadelli P (1987) Visuo-manual pursuit tracking of human two-dimensional movements. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 13:62–78

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Viviani P, Baud-Bovy G, Redolphi M (1997) Perceiving and tracking kinesthetic stimuli: further evidence of motor-perceptual interactions. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 17:198–218

    Google Scholar 

  • von Collani G (1979) An analysis of illusion component with L and ┴ -figures in active touch. Q J Exp Psychol 31:241–248

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh WD, Russell DG, Imanaka K, James B (1979) Memory for constrained and preselected movement location and distance: effect of starting position and length. J Mot Behav 11:201–214

    Google Scholar 

  • Yakimoff N, Lansky P, Mitrani L, Radil T (1989) Is the 45°-oblique a third dominant direction? Acta Neurobiol Exp 49:47–50

    CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paolo Viviani.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Baud-Bovy, G., Viviani, P. Amplitude and direction errors in kinesthetic pointing. Exp Brain Res 157, 197–214 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-004-1833-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-004-1833-1

Keywords

Navigation