Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

At what stage of manual visual reaction time does interhemispheric transmission occur: controlled or ballistic?

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Interhemispheric transfer (IT) time through the corpus callosum can be measured with a manual reaction time (RT) to lateralized visual stimuli (the so-called Poffenberger paradigm) by subtracting mean RT of faster uncrossed hemifield-hand combinations (not requiring an IT) from slower crossed combinations (requiring an IT). That the corpus callosum is involved in IT has been demonstrated by its dramatic lengthening in patients with a section of the corpus callosum. However, it is still unclear whether the signal transmitted by the corpus callosum concerns perceptual or motor stages of RT. To try and cast light on this question, in a first experiment we tested normal subjects on a partially modified Poffenberger paradigm with stop trials intermingled with go trials. In the former, subjects are supposed to refrain from responding following a stop signal (stop-signal paradigm). This paradigm can tease apart the contribution of the controlled and ballistic stages to overall RT and, used together with the Poffenberger task, enables one to assess the stage at which IT occurs. The controlled stage lies before the point of no return, i.e. the point beyond which the response cannot be inhibited, and concerns perceptual and pre-motor processes, while the ballistic stage occurs after the point of no return and concerns the motoric aspect of the response. We found that the slower responses typically obtained in the crossed conditions were more likely to be inhibited than the faster uncrossed responses and this suggests that IT occurs prior to the point of no return. Since the precise locus of the point of no return is uncertain, in a second experiment we used response force as a dependent variable reflecting the activation of the motor cortex. We found that none of the force parameters studied differed between crossed and uncrossed conditions while the temporal parameters confirmed the presence of an advantage of the uncrossed combinations. Altogether these results suggest that callosal IT of visuomotor information occurs at the stage of controlled (perceptual and pre-motor) processes and rule out the possibility of an IT at the motoric stage.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6A, B

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aglioti S, Dall’Agnola R, Girelli M, Marzi CA (1991) Bilateral hemispheric control of foot distal movements: evidence from normal subjects. Cortex 27:571–581

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Angel A (1973) Input-output relations in simple reaction time experiments. Q J Exp Psychol 25:193–200

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ashe J (1997) Force and the motor cortex. Behav Brain Res 87:255–269

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bashore TR (1981) Vocal and manual reaction time estimates of interhemispheric transmission time. Psychol Bull 89:352–368

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berlucchi G, Aglioti S, Marzi CA, Tassinari G (1995) Corpus callosum and simple visuomotor integration. Neuropsychologia 33:923–936

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlton LG, Carlton MJ, Newell KM (1987) Reaction time and response dynamics. Q J Exp Psychol 39A:337–360

    Google Scholar 

  • Cavina-Pratesi C, Bricolo E, Prior M, Marzi CA (2001) Redundancy gain in the stop-signal paradigm: implications for the locus of coactivation in simple reaction time. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 27:932–941

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chelazzi L, Marzi CA, Panozzo G, Pasqualini N, Tassinari G, Tomazzoli L (1988) Hemiretinal differences in speed of light detection in esotropic amblyopes. Vision Res 8:95–104

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheney PD, Fetz EE (1980) Functional classes of primate cortico-motoneuronal cells and their relation to active force. J Neurophysiol 44:773–791

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke JM, Zaidel E (1989) Simple reaction times to lateralized light flashes. Varieties of interhemispheric communication routes. Brain 112:849–870

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Colonius H (1990) A note on the stop-signal paradigm, or how to observe the unobservable. Psychol Rev 97:309–312

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Colonius H, Ozyurt J, Arndt PA (2001) Countermanding saccades with auditory stop signals: testing the race model. Vision Res 41:1951–1968

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cramer SC, Weisskoff RM, Schaechter JD, Nelles G, Foley M, Finklestein SP, Rosen BR (2002) Motor cortex activation is related to force of squeezing. Hum Brain Mapp 16:197–205

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Curcio CA, Sloan KR Jr, Packer O, Hendrickson AE, Kalina RE (1987) Distribution of cones in human and monkey retina: individual variability and radial asymmetry. Science 236:579–582

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Jong R, Coles MGH, Logan GD, Gratton G (1990) In search of the point of no return: the control of response processes. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 16:164–182

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Lacoste MC, Kirkpatrick JB, Ross ED (1985) Topography of the human corpus callosum. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 44:578–591

    Google Scholar 

  • Dettmers C, Fink GR, Lemon RN, Stephan KM, Passingham RE, Silbersweig D, Holmes A, Ridding MC, Brooks DJ, Frackowiak RS (1995) Relation between cerebral activity and force in the motor areas of the human brain. J Neurophysiol 74:802–815

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ehrsson HH, Fagergren A, Jonsson T, Westling G, Johansson RS, Forssberg H (2000) Cortical activity in precision- versus power-grip tasks: an fMRI study. J Neurophysiol 83:528–536

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Evarts EV (1969) Activity of pyramidal tract neurons during postural fixation. J Neurophysiol 32:375–385

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Franz EA, Miller J (2002) Effects of response readiness on reaction time and force output in people with Parkinson’s disease. Brain 125:1733–1750

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Georgopoulos AP, Ashe J, Smyrnis N, Taira M (1992) The motor cortex and the coding of force. Science 256:1692–1695

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Giray M, Ulrich R (1993) Motor coactivation revealed by response force in divided and focused attention. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 19:1278–1291

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni M, Zaidel E (1995) Channels of the corpus callosum. Evidence from simple reaction times to lateralized flashes in the normal and the split brain. Brain 118:779–788

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jaskowski P, Verleger R (1993) A clock paradigm to study the relationship between expectancy and response force. Percept Mot Skills 77:163–174

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jaskowski P, Rybarczyk K, Jaroszyk F, Lemanski D (1995) The effect of stimulus intensity on force output in simple reaction time task in humans. Acta Neurobiol Exp 55:57–64

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jennings JR, van der Molen MW, Brock K, Somsen RJM (1992) On the synchrony of stopping motor responses and delaying heartbeats. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 18:422–436

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Konishi S, Nakajima K, Uchida I, Sekihara K, Miyashita Y (1998) No-Go dominant brain activity in human inferior prefrontal cortex revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Eur J Neurosci 10:1209–1213

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Konishi S, Nakajima K, Uchida I, Kikyo H, Kameyama M, Miyashita Y (1999) Common inhibitory mechanism in human inferior prefrontal cortex revealed by event-related functional MRI. Brain 122:981–991

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lappin JS, Eriksen CW (1966) Use of a delayed signal to stop a visual reaction time response. J Exp Psychol 72:805–811

    Google Scholar 

  • Liddle PF, Kiehl KA, Smith AM (2001) Event-related fMRI study of Response Inhibition. Hum Brain Mapp 12:100–109

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Logan GD (1994) On the ability to inhibit thought and action, a users’ guide to the stop signal paradigm. In: Dagenbach D, Carr TH (eds) Inhibitory processes in attention, memory and language. Academic Press, San Diego, CA, pp 189–239

  • Logan GD, Cowan WB (1984) On the ability to inhibit thought and action: a theory of an act of control. Psychol Rev 91:295–327

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logan GD, Cowan WB, Davis KA (1984) On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 10:276–291

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marzi CA (1999) The Poffenberger paradigm: a first, simple, behavioural tool to study interhemispheric transmission in humans. Brain Res Bull 50:421–422

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marzi CA, Bisiacchi P, Nicoletti R (1991) Is interhemispheric transfer of visuomotor information asymmetric? Evidence from a meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 29:1163–1177

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marzi CA, Perani D, Tassinari G, Colleluori A, Maravita A, Miniussi C, Paulesu E, Scifo P, Fazio F (1999) Pathways of interhemispheric transfer in normals and in a split-brain subject. A positron emission tomography study. Exp Brain Res 126:451–458

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mattes S, Ulrich R (1997) Response force is sensitive to the temporal uncertainty of response stimuli. Percept Psychophys 59:1089–1097

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mattes S, Ulrich R, Miller J (2002) Response force in RT tasks: isolating effects of stimulus probability and response probability. Vis Cogn 9:477–501

    Google Scholar 

  • McGarry T, Franks IM (1997) A horse race between independent processes: evidence for a phantom point of no return in the preparation of a speeded motor response. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 23:1533–1542

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller J, Franz V, Ulrich R (1999) Effects of auditory stimulus intensity on response force in simple, go/no-go, and choice RT tasks. Percept Psychophys 61:107–119

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Milner AD, Lines CR (1982) Interhemispheric pathways in simple reaction time to lateralized light flash. Neuropsychologia 20:171–179

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mordkoff JT, Miller J, Roch AC (1996) Absence of coactivation in the motor component: evidence from psychophysiological measures of target detection. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 22:25–41

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Osman A, Kornblum S, Meyer DE (1986) The point of no return in choice reaction time: controlled and ballistic stage of response preparation. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 12:243–258

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Osman A, Kornblum S, Meyer DE (1990) Does motor programming necessitate response execution? J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 16:183–198

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Osterberg G (1935) Topography of the layer of rods and cones in the human retina. Acta Ophthalmol Suppl 61:1–102

    Google Scholar 

  • Peck KK, Sunderland A, Peters AM, Butterworth S, Clark P, Gowland PA (2001) Cerebral activation during a simple force production task: changes in the time course of the haemodynamic response. Neuroreport 12:2813–2816

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Poffenberger AT (1912) Reaction time to retinal stimulation with special reference to the time lost in conduction through nervous centres. Arch Psychol 23:1–73

    Google Scholar 

  • Rains JD (1962) Signal luminance and position effects in human reaction time. Vision Res 3:239–251

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff R (1979) Group reaction time distributions and an analysis of distribution statistics. Psychol Bull 86:446–461

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ratinckx E, Brysbaert M, Vermeulen E (2001) CRT screens may give rise to biased estimates of interhemispheric transmission time in the Poffenberger paradigm. Exp Brain Res 136:413–416

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rubia K, Russell T, Overmeyer S, Brammer MJ, Bullmore ET, Sharma T, Simmons A, Williams SCR, Giampietro V, Andrew CM, Taylor E (2001) Mapping motor inhibition: conjunctive brain activations across different version of Go/No-Go and Stop tasks. Neuroimage 13:250–261

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stancak A Jr, Luecking CH, Kristeva-Feige R (2000) Lateralization of movement-related potentials and the size of the corpus callosum. Neuroreport 11:329–332

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tettamanti M, Paulesu E, Scifo P, Maravita A, Fazio F, Perani D, Marzi CA (2002) Interhemispheric transmission of visuomotor information in humans: fMRI evidence. J Neurophysiol 88:1051–1058

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thickbroom GW, Phillips BA, Morris I, Byrnes ML, Mastaglia FL (1998) Isometric force-related activity in sensorimotor cortex measured with functional MRI. Exp Brain Res 121:59–64

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ulrich R, Wing AM (1991) A recruitment theory of force-time relations in the production of brief force pulses: the parallel force unit model. Psychol Rev 98:268–294

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ulrich R, Rinkenauer G, Miller J (1998) Effects of stimulus duration and intensity on simple reaction time and response force. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 24:915–928

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ulrich R, Mattes S, Miller J (1999) Donder’s assumption of pure insertion: an evaluation on the basis of response dynamics. Acta Psych 102:43–75

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by COFIN MURST to C.A.M. The technical assistance of Gianni Finizia and Daniele Molinari and the comments of two anonymous referees are gratefully acknowledged.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to C. A. Marzi.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Cavina-Pratesi, C., Bricolo, E., Pellegrini, B. et al. At what stage of manual visual reaction time does interhemispheric transmission occur: controlled or ballistic?. Exp Brain Res 155, 220–230 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-003-1712-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-003-1712-1

Keywords

Navigation