Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Differences in attentional bias to smoking-related, affective, and sensation-seeking cues between smokers and non-smokers: an eye-tracking study

  • Original Investigation
  • Published:
Psychopharmacology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Rationale

One of the behavioural features of tobacco use disorder is the presence of attentional bias (AB) to smoking-related stimuli. However, much of the research investigating these associations have been limited to the use of reaction-based indices.

Objectives

We aimed to investigate differences in AB to smoking, affective, and sensation-seeking cues in smokers and non-smokers using novel, free-viewing, eye-tracking technology. Secondary aims included investigating impulsivity-by-group interaction effects on AB to sensation-seeking cues.

Methods

Participants were either otherwise-healthy smokers of at least 8 cigarettes per day or otherwise-healthy non-smokers (< 100 cigarettes in their lifetime and no smoking in the past year). AB was measured using a free-viewing, eye-tracking system. Participants were presented a series of slides divided into 3 themes: smoking, affective, and sensation-seeking. Each slide contained 4 images (1 theme-related, 1 neutral, 2 competitive). Primary outcome measure was the difference in the proportion of time spent viewing the theme-related cue to neutral cue. Impulsivity was measured using a monetary delayed discounting task.

Results

The sample consisted of 50 smokers (41 ± 12 years old) and 50 age- and sex-matched non-smokers (40 ± 14 years old). Smokers spent over 2 times longer looking at smoking-related images than non-smokers (F = 25.50, p < 0.001). As well, greater impulsivity was significantly associated with increased AB to sensation-seeking cues (R2 = 0.059, F = 2.98, p = 0.04) in smokers but not non-smokers. No differences were found on AB to affective cues.

Conclusion

The eye-tracking procedure is a sensitive tool for assessing AB in smokers compared to non-smokers to both smoking and sensation-seeking cues.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Arnett J (1994) Sensation seeking: a new conceptualization and a new scale. Pers Individ Differ 16(2):289–296

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Audrain-McGovern J, Rodriguez D, Epstein LH, Cuevas J, Rodgers K, Wileyto EP (2009) Does delay discounting play an etiological role in smoking or is it a consequence of smoking? Drug Alcohol Depend 103(3):99–106

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Baker TB, Piper ME, McCarthy DE, Majeskie MR, Fiore MC (2004) Addiction motivation reformulated: an affective processing model of negative reinforcement. Psychol Rev 111(1):33

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bardo MT, Donohew R, Harrington NG (1996) Psychobiology of novelty seeking and drug seeking behavior. Behav Brain Res 77(1–2):23–43

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baschnagel JS (2013) Using mobile eye-tracking to assess attention to smoking cues in a naturalized environment. Addict Behav 38(12):2837–2840

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berey BL, Frohe TM, Pritschmann RK, Yurasek AM (2021) An examination of the acquired preparedness model among college student marijuana users. J Am Coll Heal 1–11

  • Bradley BP, Mogg K, Wright T, Field M (2003) Attentional bias in drug dependence: vigilance for cigarette-related cues in smokers. Psychol Addict Behav 17(1):66–72. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-164X.17.1.66

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brandon TH (1994) Negative affect as motivation to smoke. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 3(2):33–37

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brandon TH, Tiffany ST, Obremski KM, Baker TB (1990) Postcessation cigarette use: the process of relapse. Addict Behav 15(2):105–114

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chau SA, Herrmann N, Eizenman M, Chung J, Lanctôt KL (2015) Exploring visual selective attention towards novel stimuli in Alzheimer’s disease patients. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord Extra 5(3):492–502

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christiansen P, Schoenmakers TM, Field M (2015) Less than meets the eye: reappraising the clinical relevance of attentional bias in addiction. Addict Behav 44:43–50

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coskunpinar A, Cyders MA (2013) Impulsivity and substance-related attentional bias: a meta-analytic review. Drug Alcohol Depend 133(1):1–14

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Creswell KG, Skrzynski CJ (2021) The influence of smoking motivation on the associations among cigarette craving, attentional bias to smoking cues, and smoking behavior. Nicotine Tob Res 23(10):1727–1734

  • Doran N, Cook J, McChargue D, Myers M, Spring B (2008) Cue-elicited negative affect in impulsive smokers. Psychol Addict Behav 22(2):249

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Doran N, Khoddam R, Sanders PE, Schweizer CA, Trim RS, Myers MG (2013) A prospective study of the acquired preparedness model: the effects of impulsivity and expectancies on smoking initiation in college students. Psychol Addict Behav 27(3):714

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Drobes DJ, Elibero A, Evans DE (2006) Attentional bias for smoking and affective stimuli: a Stroop task study. Psychol Addict Behav 20(4):490

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Drobes DJ, Oliver JA, Correa JB, Evans DE (2019) Attentional bias and smoking. Neuroscience of Nicotine 145–150

  • Dumais KM, Franklin TR, Jagannathan K, Hager N, Gawrysiak M, Betts J, … Janes AC (2017) Multi-site exploration of sex differences in brain reactivity to smoking cues: consensus across sites and methodologies. Drug Alcohol Depend 178:469–476

  • Ehrman RN, Robbins SJ, Bromwell MA, Lankford ME, Monterosso JR, O’Brien CP (2002) Comparing attentional bias to smoking cues in current smokers, former smokers, and non-smokers using a dot-probe task. Drug Alcohol Depend 67(2):185–191

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eizenman M, Lawrence HY, Grupp L, Eizenman E, Ellenbogen M, Gemar M, Levitan RD (2003) A naturalistic visual scanning approach to assess selective attention in major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Res 118(2):117–128

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Field M, Cox WM (2008) Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: a review of its development, causes, and consequences. Drug Alcohol Depend 97(1–2):1–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.03.030

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Field M, Mogg K, Bradley BP (2004a) Eye movements to smoking-related cues: effects of nicotine deprivation. Psychopharmacology 173(1–2):116–123. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-003-1689-2

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Field M, Mogg K, Zetteler J, Bradley BP (2004b) Attentional biases for alcohol cues in heavy and light social drinkers: the roles of initial orienting and maintained attention. Psychopharmacology 176(1):88–93

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Field M, Munafò MR, Franken IH (2009) A meta-analytic investigation of the relationship between attentional bias and subjective craving in substance abuse. Psychol Bull 135(4):589

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Field M, Marhe R, Franken IH (2014) The clinical relevance of attentional bias in substance use disorders. CNS Spectr 19(3):225–230

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flagel SB, Akil H, Robinson TE (2009) Individual differences in the attribution of incentive salience to reward-related cues: implications for addiction. Neuropharmacology 56:139–148

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Freeman TP, Morgan CJA, Beesley T, Curran HV (2012) Drug cue induced overshadowing: selective disruption of natural reward processing by cigarette cues amongst abstinent but not satiated smokers. Psychol Med 42(1):161–171. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291711001139

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guestrin ED, Eizenman M (2006) General theory of remote gaze estimation using the pupil center and corneal reflections. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 53(6):1124–1133

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heatherton TF, Kozlowski LT, Frecker RC, Fagerstrom KO (1991) The Fagerström test for nicotine dependence: a revision of the Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnaire. Br J Addict 86(9):1119–1127

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hogarth LC, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Duka T, Dickinson A (2003) Attentional orienting towards smoking-related stimuli. Behav Pharmacol 14(2):153–160

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holmqvist K, Nyström M, Andersson R, Dewhurst R, Jarodzka H, Van de Weijer J (2011) Eye tracking: a comprehensive guide to methods and measures. OUP, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Horstmann G, Bauland A (2006) Search asymmetries with real faces: testing the anger-superiority effect. Emotion 6(2):193

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes JR, Hatsukami D (1986) Signs and symptoms of tobacco withdrawal. Arch Gen Psychiatry 43(3):289–294

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ibm II (2017) Statistics for Windows, Version 25.0. IBM Corporation, Armonk

    Google Scholar 

  • Itti L (2015) New eye-tracking techniques may revolutionize mental health screening. Neuron 88(3):442–444. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.10.033

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kahler CW, Spillane NS, Metrik J, Leventhal AM, Monti PM (2009) Sensation seeking as a predictor of treatment compliance and smoking cessation treatment outcomes in heavy social drinkers. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 93(3):285–290

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kang OS, Chang DS, Jahng GH, Kim SY, Kim H, Kim JW, … Chae Y (2012) Individual differences in smoking-related cue reactivity in smokers: an eye-tracking and fMRI study. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 38(2):285–293. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2012.04.013

  • Kaplan BA, Amlung M, Reed DD, Jarmolowicz DP, McKerchar TL, Lemley SM (2016) Automating scoring of delay discounting for the 21-and 27-item monetary choice questionnaires. Behav Anal 39(2):293–304

  • Khazaal Y, Zullino D, Billieux J (2012) The Geneva Smoking Pictures: development and preliminary validation. Eur Addict Res 18(3):103–109. https://doi.org/10.1159/000335083

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kinderman P (1994) Attentional bias, persecutory delusions and the self-concept. Br J Med Psychol 67(1):53–66

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby KN, Maraković NN (1996) Delay-discounting probabilistic rewards: rates decrease as amounts increase. Psychon Bull Rev 3(1):100–104

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby KN, Petry NM, Bickel WK (1999) Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. J Exp Psychol Gen 128(1):78

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kolokotroni K, Rodgers R, Harrison A (2012) Effects of chronic nicotine, nicotine withdrawal and subsequent nicotine challenges on behavioural inhibition in rats. Psychopharmacology 219(2):453–468

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kwak SM, Na DL, Kim G, Gye SK, Lee JH (2007) Use of eye movement to measure smokers’ attentional bias to smoking-related cues. Cyberpsychol Behav 10(2):299–304. https://doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2006.9953

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge D (2013) Attentional processing. Harvard University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Lang PJ (2005) International affective picture system (IAPS)digitized photographs, instruction manual and affective ratings. Technical report

  • Lipkus IM, Barefoot JC, Williams RB, Siegler IC (1994) Personality measures as predictors of smoking initiation and cessation in the UNC Alumni Heart Study. Health Psychol 13(2):149

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luijten M, Littel M, Franken IH (2011) Deficits in inhibitory control in smokers during a Go/NoGo task: an investigation using event-related brain potentials. PLoS One [electronic Resource] 6(4):e18898. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0018898

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lundqvist D, Flykt A, Öhman A (1998) Karolinska directed emotional faces. Cognition and Emotion

  • Mogg K, Bradley BP, Field M, De Houwer J (2003) Eye movements to smoking-related pictures in smokers: relationship between attentional biases and implicit and explicit measures of stimulus valence. Addiction 98(6):825–836. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1360-0443.2003.00392.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg K, Field M, Bradley BP (2005) Attentional and approach biases for smoking cues in smokers: an investigation of competing theoretical views of addiction. Psychopharmacology 180(2):333–341. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-005-2158-x

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perkins KA, Donny E, Caggiula AR (1999) Sex differences in nicotine effects and self-administration: review of human and animal evidence. Nicotine Tob Res 1(4):301–315. https://doi.org/10.1080/14622299050011431

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perkins KA, Gerlach D, Vender J, Grobe J, Meeker J, Hutchison S (2001) Sex differences in the subjective and reinforcing effects of visual and olfactory cigarette smoke stimuli. Nicotine Tob Res 3(2):141–150

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pinhas L, Fok K-H, Chen A, Lam E, Schachter R, Eizenman O, … Eizenman M (2014) Attentional biases to body shape images in adolescents with anorexia nervosa: An exploratory eye-tracking study. Psychiatry Res 220(1–2):519–526

  • Robinson TE, Berridge KC (1993) The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Rev 18(3):247–291

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson TE, Berridge KC (2001) Incentive-sensitization and addiction. Addiction 96(1):103–114

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor TF, De la Fuente JR, Grant M (1993) Development of the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption-II. Addiction 88(6):791–804

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Settles RF, Cyders M, Smith GT (2010) Longitudinal validation of the acquired preparedness model of drinking risk. Psychol Addict Behav 24(2):198

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sharafi Z, Sharif B, Guéhéneuc YG, Begel A, Bednarik R, Crosby M (2020) A practical guide on conducting eye tracking studies in software engineering. Empir Softw Eng 25(5):3128–3174

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, … Dunbar GC (1998) The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry 59(20):22–33

  • Shiffman S, Waters AJ (2004) Negative affect and smoking lapses: a prospective analysis. J Consult Clin Psychol 72(2):192

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schuh KJ, Stitzer ML (1995) Desire to smoke during spaced smoking intervals. Psychopharmacology 120(3):289–295. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02311176

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simonson J, Sánchez O, Arger C, Mezulis AH (2012) Integrating affective and cognitive vulnerabilities to depression: examining individual differences in cognitive responses to induced stress. Cogn Ther Res 36(5):474–482

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith G, Anderson K (2001) Adolescent risk for alcohol problems as acquired preparedness: a model and suggestions for intervention. Adolesc Alcohol Subs Abuse 1:109–144

    Google Scholar 

  • Spitzer R, Gibbon M, Williams J (1997) User’s guide for the structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders SCID-I: clinician version. American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC

    Google Scholar 

  • Stacy AW, Newcomb MD, Bentler PM (1993) Cognitive motivations and sensation seeking as long-term predictors of drinking problems. J Soc Clin Psychol 12(1):1–24

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Storey JD (2011) False discovery rate. International Encyclopedia of Statistical Science 1:504–508

  • Sturm V, Cassel D, Eizenman M (2011) Objective estimation of visual acuity with preferential looking. Invest Ophthalmol vis Sci 52(2):708–713

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson ER (2007) Development and validation of an internationally reliable short-form of the positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS). J Cross Cult Psychol 38(2):227–242

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tiffany ST, Drobes DJ (1991) The development and initial validation of a questionnaire on smoking urges. Br J Addict 86(11):1467–1476

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Waters AJ, Feyerabend C (2000) Determinants and effects of attentional bias in smokers. Psychol Addict Behav 14(2):111

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Waters AJ, Shiffman S, Bradley BP, Mogg K (2003) Attentional shifts to smoking cues in smokers. Addiction 98(10):1409–1417

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilcockson TDW, Pothos EM, Osborne AM, Crawford TJ (2021) Top-down and bottom-up attentional biases for smoking-related stimuli: comparing dependent and non-dependent smokers. Addict Behav 118:106886

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wingo T, Nesil T, Choi J-S, Li MD (2016) Novelty seeking and drug addiction in humans and animals: from behavior to molecules. J Neuroimmune Pharmacol 11(3):456–470

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This research is funded by Global Research Awards for Nicotine Dependence (GRAND), a peer-reviewed research grant competition funded by Pfizer Inc (Zawertailo (GRAND2012) WS2391913).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Laurie Zawertailo.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors report the following potential competing interests: LZ and PS have received investigator-initiated funding from Pfizer Canada and Pfizer Inc. through the Global Research Awards in Nicotine Dependence (GRAND) that is independent of the work written in this manuscript.

Additional information

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 4225 KB)

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Rahmani, N., Chung, J., Eizenman, M. et al. Differences in attentional bias to smoking-related, affective, and sensation-seeking cues between smokers and non-smokers: an eye-tracking study. Psychopharmacology 239, 3711–3721 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-022-06245-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-022-06245-y

Keywords

Navigation