Advertisement

Psychopharmacology

, Volume 231, Issue 11, pp 2371–2373 | Cite as

It’s timely and time for the change: comments on Peck and Ranaldi

  • Joseph R. TroisiIIEmail author
Commentary

Substance abuse poses a significant financial burden on society in terms of health care cost and other economic indices—an argument made by many engaged in basic translational behavioral/psychopharmacological animal research that targets chemical addiction (e.g., Peck and Ranaldi 2014; Troisi 2013). A recent theoretical paper by Peck and Ranaldi (2014) addressed clinical translations of basic behavioral treatment strategies for drug abuse, challenged their clinical efficacy, and suggested improvements. Their publication is timely because similar arguments, conclusions, and suggestions have recently been proposed (Troisi 2013). This author, therefore, felt compelled to comment.

Among several treatments noted in P and R’s paper, Pavlovian-based cue-exposure therapy (CET) and operant-based contingency management (CM) were highlighted. Indeed, it is well established that CET has evidenced little clinical efficacy because of at least three common observations reported in the Pavlovian and operant animal learning literatures: Spontaneous recovery (the return of the response following a delay after extinction), reinstatement (the consequent increase in responding due to non-contingent presentation of the drug reinforcer/US or stressor following extinction), and context renewal (the return of the response following extinction in a novel context) are culprits of relapse (Bouton and Swartzentruber 1991; Bouton et al. 2012; Conklin and Tiffany 2002; Troisi 2013). In regard to CET, several points that follow were not made by P and R that would have augmented their argument. First, extinction in CET is incomplete. Users do not self-administer a placebo thereby leaving the self-administration response intact. Furthermore, only one to nine sessions have been reported (Conklin and Tiffany 2002). Second, as previously discussed (Troisi 2013), drug-associated CSs can evoke cue-reactivity/craving and promote relapse; however, individuals often relapse without craving, and many who crave do not relapse—and might actually benefit from craving (Drummond 2001; Niaura et al. 1988; Rohsenow and Monti 1999; Tiffany and Carter 1998; Tracy 1994). Finally, operant discriminative stimuli (SD’s) have more robust modulatory effects on extinguished drug self-administration responses than Pavlovian drug-paired CSs (Di Ciano and Everitt 2003; Hogarth and Chase 2012; McFarland and Ettenberg 1997). In view of these points, a Pavlovian account of drug-seeking, drug-taking, drug-relapse, and treatment does not suffice. This assertion has been implied elsewhere—and long ago (see Greeley and Ryan 1995; McFarland and Ettenberg 1997). Greeley and Ryan (1995), and later Conklin and Tiffany (2002), suggested that greater emphasis be placed on the role of the operant SD in regulating drug-seeking and drug-taking behavior. Unfortunately, Pavlovian interpretations and models prevail in the literature (i.e., role of the CS), perhaps because of their simplicity and methodological ease in contrast to operant methodology.

By comparison, the operant-based CM has greater long-term effectiveness than CET. In CM, non-drug use (as measured by chronic drug testing) is positively reinforced by vouchers, which are exchanged for community goods (Higgins and Silverman 2008; Silverman et al. 2004; Troisi 2013 review). As noted by P and R, however, these effects are only evident by increasing the value of the vouchers over time. When the vouchers are discontinued, relapse rates increase. Crucially, although not addressed by P and R, another behavioral approach referred to as therapeutic work environment (TWE) is more effective. TWE not only reinforces drug abstinence but also systematically shapes and maintains work-related job skills in low-income drug abusers (Silverman et al. 2004, 2008; DeFulio et al. 2012; DeFulio and Silverman 2011). In one investigation, Silverman et al. (2002) reported that up to 60 % of their sample was drug-free 3 years following completion of the study.

Peck and Ranaldi suggest that promoting alternative behaviors through environmental enrichment (EE) may be clinically important. They also suggested that continued exposure to drug cues and drug availability with punitive consequences for self-administration, which they refer to as abstinence conflict paradigm, may have greater ecological validity as an animal paradigm to simulate better drug treatment programs. P and R highlight cocaine self-administration data from their lab that demonstrated how EE in rats attenuated recovery of extinguished responding. Similar effects have also been reported with exercise in rats and mice (Zlebnik et al. 2010; Mustroph et al. 2011). It might be even more clinically informative to combine EE and the abstinence conflict paradigm in a choice procedure.

One additional behavioral phenomenon reported in the animal learning literature that is likely to be an obstacle for the effectiveness of CM and TWE is behavioral resurgence. In a behavioral resurgence paradigm with rats, the original drug-taking behavior is extinguished concomitant with establishing a novel behavior that is maintained by an alternative non-drug reinforcer (e.g., sucrose). If the alternative behavior is extinguished, the original behavior that was previously maintained by the drug recovers (i.e., resurges). Resurgence has been reported to be a form of context renewal (Bouton et al. 2012; c.f., Shahan and Sweeney 2011). Explicit concerns have been made by P and R as well as others (Bouton et al. 2012; Troisi 2013) regarding the potential for behavioral resurgence (relapse) following CM and TWE if funding is suspended—or following job loss. Under such conditions, alternative pro-social behaviors for alternative sources of non-drug reinforcement would be required to minimize relapse—especially if such novel repertoires were to be executed under specific internal motivating states (emotions, stressors, thirst, hunger, fatigue) that previously coincided with drug seeking and taking. The translational importance of the EE results from the animal literature and clinical treatment research is a significant step in this direction.

Peck and Ranaldi made a more than compelling argument for the need for novel and ecologically valid animal learning paradigms to simulate drug abuse and relapse—paradigms that integrate many therapeutic domains. As previously discussed, drug-seeking, drug-taking, and relapse is the culmination of an overlapping sensory gestalt of environmental discriminative stimuli (that set the occasion for drug availability and self-administration) and internal motivating states that coincide before, during, and after drug use (Troisi 2013). Models that simulate complex interactions among Pavlovian and operant stimulus control phenomena may lead to better clinical translation and long-term success of behavioral therapy for drug abuse.

Notes

Acknowledgments

The author extends special thanks to Ms. Robin Allard for her copy edits.

Conflict of interest

There is no conflict of interest—financial or otherwise.

References

  1. Bouton ME, Swartzentruber D (1991) Sources of relapse after extinction in Pavlovian and instrumental learning. Clin Psychol Rev 11:123–140CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bouton ME, Winterbauer NE, Vurbic D (2012) Context and extinction: mechanisms of relapse in drug self-administration. In: Haselgrove M, Hogarth L (eds) Clinical applications of learning theory. Psychology Press, East Sussex, pp 103–133Google Scholar
  3. Conklin CA, Tiffany ST (2002) Applying extinction research and theory to cue-exposure addiction treatments. Addiction 97:155–167PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. DeFulio A, Silverman K (2011) Employment-based abstinence reinforcement as a maintenance intervention for the treatment of cocaine dependence: post-intervention outcomes. Addiction 106:960–967. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03364.x PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. DeFulio A, Everly JJ, Leoutsakos JS, Umbricht A, Fingerhood M, Bigelow GE, Silverman K (2012) Employment-based reinforcement of adherence to an FDA approved extended release formulation of naltrexone in opioid-dependent adults: a randomized controlled trial. Drug Alcohol Depend 120:48–54. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.06.023 PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Di Ciano P, Everitt BJ (2003) Differential control over drug-seeking behavior by drug-associated conditioned reinforcers and discriminative stimuli predictive of drug availability. Behav Neurosci 117:952–960. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.5.952 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Drummond DC (2001) Theories of drug craving, ancient and mondern. Addiction 96:33–46Google Scholar
  8. Greeley J, Ryan C (1995c. In: Drummond D, Tiffany ST, Glautier S, Remington B, Drummond D, Tiffany V,… Remington B (eds), Addictive behaviour: cue exposure theory and practice. Oxford, Wiley, pp. 119–136Google Scholar
  9. Higgins S, Silverman K (2008) Contingency management. The American Psychiatric Publishing textbook of substance abuse treatment, 4th edn. American Psychiatric Publishing, Arlington, pp 387–399Google Scholar
  10. Hogarth L, Chase HW (2012) Evaluating psychological markers for human nicotine dependence: tobacco choice, extinction, and Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 20:213–224. doi: 10.1037/a0027203 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. McFarland K, Ettenberg AA (1997) Reinstatement of drug-seeking behavior produced by heroin-predictive environmental stimuli. Psychopharmacology 131:86–92PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Mustroph ML, Stobaugh DJ, Miller DS, DeYoung EK, Rhodes JS (2011) Wheel running can accelerate or delay extinction of conditioned place preference for cocaine in male C57BL/6J mice, depending on timing of wheel access. Eur J Neurosci 34:1161–1169PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Niaura RS, Rohsenow DJ, Binkoff JA, Monti PM, Pedraza M, Abrams DB (1988) Relevance of cue reactivity to understanding alcohol and smoking relapse. J Abnorm Psychol 97:133–152. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.97.2.133 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Peck JA, Ranaldi R (2014) Drug abstinence: exploring animal models and behavioral treatment strategies. Psychopharmacology. doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3517-2 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Rohsenow DJ, Monti PM (1999) Does urge to drink predict relapse after treatment? Alcohol Res Health 23:225–232PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Shahan TA, Sweeney MM (2011) A model of resurgence based on behavioral momentum theory. J Exp Anal Behav 95:91–108PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Silverman K, Svikis D, Wong CJ, Hampton J, Stitzer ML, Bigelow GE (2002) A reinforcement-based therapeutic workplace for the treatment of drug abuse: three- year abstinence outcomes. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 10:228–240. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.10.3.228 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Silverman K, Robles E, Mudric T, Bigelow G, Stitzer M (2004) A randomized trial of long-term reinforcement of cocaine abstinence in methadone-maintained patients who inject drugs. J Consult Clin Psychol 72:839–854PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Silverman K, Roll J, Higgins S (2008) Introduction to the special issue on the behavior analysis and treatment of drug addiction. J Appl Behav Anal 41:471–480PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Tiffany ST, Carter BL (1998) Is craving the source of compulsive drug use? J Psychopharmacol 12:23–30PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Tracy JI (1994) Assessing the relationship between craving and relapse. Drug Alcohol Rev 13:71–77. doi: 10.1080/09595239400185751 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Troisi JR II (2013) Perhaps more consideration of Pavlovian–operant interaction may improve the clinical efficacy of behaviorally based drug treatment programs. Psychol Rec 63:863–894. doi: 10.11133/j.tpr.2013.63.4.010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Zlebnik NE, Anker JJ, Gliddon LA, Carroll ME (2010) Reduction of extinction and reinstatement of cocaine seeking by wheel running in female rats. Psychopharmacology 209:113–125PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Saint Anselm CollegeManchesterUSA

Personalised recommendations