Advertisement

Psychopharmacology

, Volume 203, Issue 4, pp 745–752 | Cite as

Enhancement of suggestibility and imaginative ability with nitrous oxide

  • M. G. Whalley
  • G. B. Brooks
Original Investigation

Abstract

Rationale

Imaginative suggestibility, a trait closely related to hypnotic suggestibility, is modifiable under some circumstances. Nitrous oxide (laughing gas) is commonly used for sedation in dentistry and is reported to be more effective when combined with appropriate suggestions.

Objective

The aim of this study was to determine whether nitrous oxide inhalation alters imaginative suggestibility and imagery vividness.

Methods

Thirty participants were tested twice in a within-subjects design, once during inhalation of 25% nitrous oxide and once during inhalation of air plus oxygen. Before the study, participants’ expectancies regarding the effects of nitrous oxide were assessed. Participants were blinded to drug administration. During each session, participants were verbally administered detailed measures of imagination and suggestibility: the Sheehan–Betts Quality of Mental Imagery scale and the Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale Form C, minus the hypnotic induction.

Results

Imaginative suggestibility and imaginative ability (imagery vividness) were both elevated in the nitrous oxide condition. This effect was unrelated to participants’ expectations regarding the effects of the drug.

Conclusions

Nitrous oxide increased imaginative suggestibility and imaginative ability. Possible explanations of these findings are discussed with respect to the effects of N-methyl-d-aspartate antagonists and to other pharmacological effects upon suggestibility and imagination.

Keywords

Suggestibility Nitrous oxide Imagination Imaginative ability Imagery Hypnosis Hypnotisability Suggestion Vividness NMDA 

Notes

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank David Oakley, Irving Kirsch, and Celia Morgan for their helpful comments during the preparation of this manuscript.

References

  1. Allen WA (1972) Relative analgesia: an introductory note. Br Dent J 133:25–26CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Barber J, Donaldson D, Ramras S, Allen GD (1979) The relationship between nitrous oxide conscious sedation and the hypnotic state. J Am Dent Assoc 99(4):624–626PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Benham G, Bowers S, Nash M, Muenchen R (1998) Self-fulfilling prophecy and hypnotic response are not the same thing. J Pers Soc Psychol 75:1604–1613. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.75.6.1604 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Bertrand LD (1989) The assessment and modification of hypnotic susceptibility. In: Spanos NP, Chaves JF (eds) Hypnosis: the cognitive–behavioral perspective. Prometheus Books, Buffalo (NY), pp 18–31Google Scholar
  5. Betts GH (1909) The distribution and functions of mental imagery. Teachers College, Columbia University, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  6. Bingham GD (1964) Rapid hypnosis by using nitrous oxide. Am J Clin Hypn 7:226–228PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Brown RJ, Oakley DA (2004) An integrative cognitive theory of hypnosis and high hypnotisability. In: Heap M, Brown RJ, Oakley DA (eds) The highly hypnotizable person. Routledge, London, pp 152–186Google Scholar
  8. Cass N, Cass L (1994) Pharmacology for anaesthetists. Churchill Livingstone, EdinburghGoogle Scholar
  9. Crawford HJ (1982) Hypnotizability, daydreaming styles, imagery vividness, and absorption: a multidimensional study. J Pers Soc Psychol 42:915–926CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. de Groh M (1989) Correlates of hypnotic susceptibility. In: Spanos NP, Chaves JF (eds) Hypnosis: the cognitive–behavioural perspective. Prometheus, Buffalo, NY, pp 32–63Google Scholar
  11. Derbyshire SWG, Whalley MG, Stenger VA, Oakley DA (2004) Cerebral activation during hypnotically induced and imagined pain. Neuroimage 23:392–401. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.04.033 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Dienes Z, Perner J (2007) Executive control without conscious awareness: the cold control theory of hypnosis. In: Jamieson GA (ed) Hypnosis and conscious states. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  13. Eysenck HJ, Rees WL (1945) States of heightened suggestibility: narcosis. J Ment Sci 91:301–310Google Scholar
  14. Faymonville ME, Mambourg PH, Joris J, Vrijens B, Fissette J, Albert A, Lamy M (1997) Psychological approaches during conscious sedation. Hypnosis versus stress reducing strategies: a prospective randomized study. Pain 73:361–367. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(97)00122-X CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Franks NP, Lieb WR (1998) A serious target for laughing gas. Nat Med 4(4):383–384. doi: 10.1038/nm0498-383 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Gandhi B, Oakley DA (2005) Does ‘hypnosis’ by any other name smell as sweet? The efficacy of ‘hypnotic’ inductions depends on the label ‘hypnosis’. Conscious Cogn 14:304–315. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2004.12.004 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Gibson HB, Corcoran ME, Curran JD (1977) Hypnotic susceptibility and personality: the consequences of diazepam and the sex of the subjects. Br J Psychol 68:51–59PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Glisky ML, Tataryn DJ, Kihlstrom JF (1995) Hypnotizability and mental imagery. Int J Clin Exp Hypn 43:34–54CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Gorasini DR (2004) Enhancing hypotisability. In: Heap M, Brown RJ, Oakley DA (eds) The highly hypnotizable person. Routledge, London, pp 213–239Google Scholar
  20. Gorassini DR, Spanos NP (1989) The Carleton Skill Training Programme for modifying hypnotic suggestibility: original version and variations. In: Kirsch I, Capafons A, Amigó S, Cardeña-Buelna E (eds) Clinical hypnosis and self-regulation therapy: a cognitive–behavioural perspective. American Psychological Association Books, Washington, DC, pp 141–77Google Scholar
  21. Hilgard ER (1986) Divided consciousness: multiple controls in human thought and action (expanded edition). Wiley, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  22. Hilgard ER, Hilgard JR (1975) Hypnosis in the relief of pain. Brunner/Mazel, Levittown, PAGoogle Scholar
  23. Holmes EA, Matthews A (2005) Mental imagery and emotion: a special relationship? Emotion 5:489–497. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.4.489 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Jevtovic-Todorovic V, Todorovic SM, Mennerick S, Powell S, Dikranian K, Benshoff N, Zorumski CF, Olney JW (1998) Nitrous oxide (laughing gas) is an NMDA antagonist, neuroprotectant and neurotoxin. Nat Med 4:460–463. doi: 10.1038/nm0498-460 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Kelly SF, Fisher S, Kelly RJ (1978) Effects of cannabis intoxication on primary suggestibility. Psychopharmacology 56:217–219CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Kihlstrom JF (2007) Modified version of the SHSS:C. Retrieved September 30th, 2008, from http://socrates.berkeley.edu/~kihlstrm/PDFfiles/Hypnotizability/SHSSC%20Script.pdf
  27. Kirsch I (1985) Response expectancy as a determinant of experience and behaviour. Am Psychol 40:1189–1202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Kirsch I, Braffman W (2001) Imaginative suggestibility and hypnotizability. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 10(2):57–61. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00115 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Kosslyn SM, Thompson WL, Constantini-Ferrando MF, Alpert NM, Speigel D (2000) Hypnotic visual illusion alters color processing in the brain. Am J Psychiatr 157:1279–1284. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.8.1279 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Kosslyn SM, Ganis G, Thompson WL (2001) Neural foundations of imagery. Nat Rev Neurosci 2:635–642CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Lippe HT (1944) Nitrous oxide analgesia in cavity preparation. Temple Dental Review 14:7Google Scholar
  32. McAvine LP, Robertson IH (2006-2007) Measuring visual imagery ability: a review. Imagin Cogn Pers 26:191–211. doi: 10.2190/3515-8169-24J8-7157 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Morgan CJA, Mofeez A, Brandner B, Bromley L, Curren HV (2004) Acute effects of ketamine on memory systems and psychotic symptoms in healthy volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology 29:208–218. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300342 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Orne MT (1962) On the social psychology of the psychological experiment: with particular reference to demand characteristics and their implications. Am Psychol 17(11):776–783CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Ott U (2007) States of absorption: in search of neurobiological foundations. In: Jamieson GA (ed) Hypnosis and conscious states. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  36. Pekala RJ, Kumar VK (2007) An empirical-phenomenological approach to quantifying consciousness and states of consciousness: with particular reference to understanding the nature of hypnosis. In: Jamieson GA (ed) Hypnosis and conscious states. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  37. Perry C, Nadon R, Button J (1992) The measurement of hypnotic ability. In: Fromm E, Nash M (eds) Contemporary hypnosis research. Guilford, New York, pp 459–490Google Scholar
  38. Seladin HM (1947) Practical anesthesia for dentistry and oral surgery. Lea & Febiger, PhiladelphiaGoogle Scholar
  39. Sheehan PW (1967) A shortened form of Betts’ questionnaire upon mental imagery. J Clin Psychol 23:386–389CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Shor RE, Orne EC (1962) Harvard group scale of hypnotic susceptibility: Form A. Consulting Psychologists, Palo Alto, CaliforniaGoogle Scholar
  41. Simons D, Potter C, Temple G (2007) Hypnosis and communication in dental practice. Quintessence, LondonGoogle Scholar
  42. Sjoberg BM, Hollister LE (1965) The effects of psychomimetic drugs on primary suggestibility. Psychopharmacology 8:251–262CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Spanos NP (1986) Hypnosis and the modification of hypnotic susceptibility: a social psychological perspective. In: Naish P (ed) What is hypnosis? Open University Press, Philadelphia, pp 85–120Google Scholar
  44. Sutcliffe JP, Perry CW, Sheehan PW (1970) Relation of some aspects of imagery and fantasy to hypnotic susceptibility. J Abnorm Psychology 76:279–287CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Szechtman H, Woody E, Bowers K, Nahmias C (1998) Where the imaginal appears real: a positron emission tomography study of auditory hallucinations. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95:1956–1960. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.4.1956 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Vickery AR, Kirsch I (1991) The effects of brief expectancy manipulations on hypnotic responsiveness. Contemp Hypn 8:167–171Google Scholar
  47. Wagman R, Stewart CG (1974) Visual imagery and hypnotic susceptibility. Percept Mot Skills 38:815–822PubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Walker DJ, Zacny JP (2005) Subjective effects of nitrous oxide. In: Earlywine M (ed) Mind-altering drugs: the science of subjective experience. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  49. Weitzenhoffer AM, Hilgard ER (1959) Stanford hypnotic susceptibility scale, Forms A and B. Consulting Psychologists, Palo Alto, CAGoogle Scholar
  50. Weitzenhoffer AM, Hilgard ER (1962) Stanford hypnotic susceptibility scale, Form C. Consulting Psychologists, Palo Alto, CAGoogle Scholar
  51. Weitzenhoffer AM, Sjoberg BM (1961) Suggestibility with and without ‘induction of hypnosis’. J Nerv Ment Dis 132:204–220CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Westcott TB, Rosenstock E (1976) Reliability of two measures of imagery. Percept Mot Skills 42:1037–1038Google Scholar
  53. White K, Ashton R, Law H (1974) Factor analyses of the shortened form of Betts’ Questionnaire Upon Mental Imagery. Aust J Psychol 26:183–190CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Hypnosis Unit, Department of PsychologyUniversity College LondonLondonUK

Personalised recommendations