Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Alcohol attentional bias: drinking salience or cognitive impairment?

  • Original Investigation
  • Published:
Psychopharmacology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Objective

This study evaluated whether alcohol attentional bias is an artifact of excessive drinkers’ impaired cognitive functioning, which adversely affects their performance on the classic Stroop test (a measure of inhibitory control) and the Shipley Institute of Living Scale (SILS; a measure of verbal and abstraction ability). Both tests measure aspects of executive cognitive functioning (ECF).

Methods

Social drinkers (N=87) and alcohol-dependent drinkers (N=47) completed a measure of alcohol consumption, classic and alcohol-related Stroop tests, and the SILS.

Results

A multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) showed that the dependent drinkers were poorer on the cognitive measures (SILS scores and classic Stroop interference) and had greater alcohol attentional bias than the social drinkers. An analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) in which the cognitive measures were controlled showed that the dependent drinkers’ greater alcohol attentional bias was not an artifact of their poorer cognitive performance.

Conclusion

The results are discussed in terms of cognitive–motivational models, which suggest that excessive drinking sensitizes alcohol abusers’ attentional responsiveness to alcohol-related stimuli to a degree that exceeds the adverse effects of alcohol on their general cognitive functioning.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The stimuli in the four blocks were as follows. Block 1: beer, whisky, scotch, liquor, tequila, bar, rum; gate, shed cupboard, tap, fence, toilet, ceiling. Block 2: shot, shorts, vodka, pint, liqueur, alcohol, bourbon; garden, shelf, alcove, carpet, chimney, radiator, stove. Block 3: wine, bitter, stout, pub, brandy, champagne, mead; hall, tail, patio, drainpipe, socket, doorknob, roof. Block 4: drink, sherry, cider, booze, spirit, gin, cocktail; bath, stairs, balcony, porch, lamp, fireplace, sink.

  2. An alcopop is an alcoholic beverage that came on the market in the United Kingdom in 1995. It is a sweet, effervescent fruit drink, usually with about 5% of alcohol by volume.

  3. In Cohen (1992), an f represents the effect size for an ANOVA model; values of 0.10, 0.25, and 0.40 are defined as small, medium, and large, respectively.

References

  • Adleman NE, Menon V, Blasey CM, White CD, Warsofsky IS, Glover GH, Reiss AL (2002) A developmental fMRI study of the Stroop color–word task. Neuroimage 16(1):61–75

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Algom D, Chajut E, Lev S (2004) A rational look at the emotional Stroop phenomenon: a generic slowdown, not a Stroop effect. J Exp Psychol Gen 133(3):323–338

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Allman JM, Hakeem A, Erwin JM, Nimchinsky E, Hof P (2001) The anterior cingulate cortex. The evolution of an interface between emotion and cognition. Ann N Y Acad Sci 935:107–117

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ardouin C, Pillon B, Peiffer E, Bejjani P, Limousin P, Damier P, Arnulf I, Benabid AL, Agid Y, Pollak P (1999) Bilateral subthalamic or pallidal stimulation for Parkinson’s disease affects neither memory nor executive functions: a consecutive series of 62 patients. Ann Neurol 46(2):217–223

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen HR, Piepenbrock R, van Rijn H (1993) The CELEX lexical database (CD-ROM). Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA

  • Bantick SJ, Wise RG, Ploghaus A, Clare S, Smith SM, Tracey I (2002) Imaging how attention modulates pain in humans using functional MRI. Brain 125(Pt 2):310–319

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer D, Cox WM (1998) Alcohol related words are distracting to both alcohol abusers and non-abusers in the Stroop colour-naming task. Addiction 93(10):1539–1542

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Blume AW, Marlatt GA, Schmaling KB (2000) Executive cognitive function and heavy drinking behavior among college students. Psychol Addict Behav 14(3):299–302

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brown M, Besner D (2001) On a variant of Stroop’s paradigm: which cognitions press your buttons? Mem Cognit 29(6):903–904

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brown SA, Tapert SF, Granholm E, Delis DC (2000) Neurocognitive functioning of adolescents: effects of protracted alcohol use. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 24:164–171

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bunge SA, Klingberg T, Jacobsen RB, Gabrieli JD (2000) A resource model of the neural basis of executive working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97(7):3573–3578

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bush G, Frazier JA, Rauch SL, Seidman LJ, Whalen PJ, Jenike MA, Rosen BR, Biederman J (1999) Anterior cingulate cortex dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder revealed by fMRI and the Counting Stroop. Biol Psychiatry 45(12):1542–1552

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bush G, Luu P, Posner MI (2000) Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 4(6):215–222

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cabeza R, Nyberg L (2000) Imaging cognition II: an empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. J Cogn Neurosci 12(1):1–47

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Carter CS, Macdonald AM, Botvinick M, Ross LL, Stenger A, Noll D, Cohen JD (2000) Parsing executive processes: strategic vs. evaluative functions of the anterior cingulate cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97(4):1944–1948

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cedrus Corporation (1999) SuperLab Pro SKD (version 2.1) [software]. Cedrus Corporation, San Pedro, CA, USA

  • Childress AR, Mozley PD, McElgin W, Fitzgerald J, Reivich M, O’Brien CP (1999) Limbic activation during cue-induced cocaine craving. Am J Psychiatry 156(1):11–18

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ciesielski KT, Waldorf AV, Jung RE (1995) Anterior brain deficits in chronic alcoholism—cause or effect? J Nerv Ment Dis 183(12):756–761

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen J (1992) A power primer. Psychol Bull 112(1):155–159

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Connor PD, Sampson PD, Bookstein FL, Barr HM, Streissguth AP (2000) Direct and indirect effects of prenatal alcohol damage on executive function. Dev Neuropsychol 18(3):331–354

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cox WM (2000) Alcohol Use Questionnaire. Unpublished questionnaire. University of Wales, Bangor

  • Cox WM, Klinger E (1988) A motivational model of alcohol use. J Abnorm Psychol 97:168–180

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cox WM, Klinger E (1990) Incentive motivation, affective change, and alcohol use: a model. In: Cox WM (ed) Why people drink: parameters of alcohol as a reinforcer. Gardner, New York, pp 291–314

    Google Scholar 

  • Cox WM, Klinger E (2004) A motivational model of alcohol use: Determinants of use and change. In: Cox WM, Klinger E (eds) Handbook of motivational counseling: concepts, approaches, and assessment. Wiley, Chichester, UK, pp 121–138

    Google Scholar 

  • Cox WM, Fadardi JS, Pothos EM (2005) The alcohol-Stroop test: theoretical considerations and procedural recommendations. Psychol Bull (in press)

  • Crombez G, Hermans D, Adriaensen H (2000) The emotional Stroop task and chronic pain: what is threatening for chronic pain sufferers? Eur J Pain 4(1):37–44

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dao-Castellana MH, Samson Y, Legault F, Martinot JL, Aubin HJ, Crouzel C, Feldman L, Barrucand D, Rancurel G, Feline A, Syrota A (1998) Frontal dysfunction in neurologically normal chronic alcoholic subjects: metabolic and neuropsychological findings. Psychol Med 28(5):1039–1048

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson RJ (1998) Affective style and affective disorders: perspectives from affective neuroscience. Cogn Emot 12(3):307–330

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson RJ, Irwin W (1999) The functional neuroanatomy of emotion and affective style. Trends Cogn Sci 3(1):11–21

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deckel AW, Bauer L, Hesselbrock V (1995) Anterior brain dysfunctioning as a risk factor in alcoholic behaviors. Addiction 90(10):1323–1334

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Derbyshire SW, Vogt BA, Jones AK (1998) Pain and Stroop interference tasks activate separate processing modules in anterior cingulate cortex. Exp Brain Res 118(1):52–60

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • de Ruiter C, Brosschot JF (1994) The emotional Stroop interference effect in anxiety: attentional bias or cognitive avoidance? Behav Res Ther 32(3):315–319

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duka T, Townshend JM (2004) The priming effect of alcohol pre-load on attentional bias to alcohol-related stimuli. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 176(3–4):353–361

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fadda F, Rossetti Z (1998) Chronic ethanol consumption: from neuroadaptation to neurodegeneration. Prog Neurobiol 56:385–431

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fuentes LJ (2001) Selective attention deficit in schizophrenia. Rev Neurol 32(4):387–391

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • George MS, Ketter TA, Parekh PI, Rosinsky N, Ring HA, Pazzaglia PJ, Marangell LB, Callahan AM, Post RM (1997) Blunted left cingulate activation in mood disorder subjects during a response interference task (the Stroop). J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 9(1):55–63

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • George MS, Anton RF, Bloomer C, Teneback C, Drobes DJ, Lorberbaum JP, Nahas Z, Vincent DJ (2001) Activation of prefrontal cortex and anterior thalamus in alcoholic subjects on exposure to alcohol-specific cues. Arch Gen Psychiatry 58(4):345–352

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Grant I, Mohns L (1975) Chronic cerebral effects of alcohol and drug abuse. Int J Addict 10(5):883–920

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Grant S, London ED, Newlin DB, Villemagne VL, Liu X, Contoreggi C, Phillips RL, Kimes AS, Margolin A (1996) Activation of memory circuits during cue-elicited cocaine craving. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 93(21):12040–12045

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Green MW, Rogers PJ (1993) Selective attention to food and body shape words in dieters and restrained nondieters. Int J Eat Disord 14(4):515–517

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Heather N, Peters Timothy J, Stockwell T (2001) International handbook of alcohol dependence and problems. Wiley, Chichester

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoaken PN, Giancola PR, Pihl RO (1998) Executive cognitive functions as mediators of alcohol-related aggression. Alcohol Alcohol 33(1):47–54

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jones BT, Jones BC, Smith H, Copely N (2003) A flicker paradigm for inducing change blindness reveals alcohol and cannabis information processing biases in social users. Addiction 98:235–244

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kilts CD, Schweitzer JB, Quinn CK, Gross RE, Faber TL, Muhammad F, Ely TD, Hoffman JM, Drexler KP (2001) Neural activity related to drug craving in cocaine addiction. Arch Gen Psychiatry 58(4):334–341

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kinderman P (1994) Attentional bias, persecutory delusions and the self-concept. Br J Med Psychol 67(Pt 1):53–66

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kindt M, Bierman D, Brosschot JF (1997) Cognitive bias in spider fear and control children: assessment of emotional interference by a card format and a single-trial format of the Stroop task. J Exp Child Psychol 66(2):163–179

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • King AC, Bernardy NC, Hauner K (2003) Stressful events, personality, and mood disturbance: Gender differences in alcoholics and problem drinkers. Addict Behav 28(1):171–187

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klinger E (1975) Consequences of commitment to and disengagement from incentives. Psychol Rev 82(1):1–25

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klinger E (1977). Meaning and void: inner experience and the incentives in people’s lives. University of Minnesota Press, Minneapolis

    Google Scholar 

  • Klinger E (1987). Current concerns and disengagement from incentives. In: Halisch F, Kuhl J (eds) Motivation, attention, and volition. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg, New York, pp 337–347

    Google Scholar 

  • Klinger E (1996) The contents of thoughts: interference as the downside of adaptive normal mechanisms in thought flow. In: Sarason IG, Sarason BR, Pierce GR (eds) Cognitive interference: theories, methods, and findings. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 3–23

    Google Scholar 

  • Klinger E, Cox WM (2004) Motivation and the theory of current concerns. In: Cox WM, Klinger E (eds) Handbook of motivational counseling: concepts, approaches, and assessment. Wiley, Chichester, UK, pp 3–27

    Google Scholar 

  • Koob GF (2003) Alcoholism: allostasis and beyond. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 27(2):232–243

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kucera H, Francis WN (1967) Computational analysis of present-day American English. Brown University Press, Providence, RI

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawton-Craddock A, Nixon SJ, Tivis R (2003) Cognitive efficiency in stimulant abusers with and without alcohol dependence. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 27(3):457–464

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lyvers M (2000) “Loss of control” in alcoholism and drug addiction: a neuroscientific interpretation. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 8(2):225–249

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod CM (1991) Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychol Bull 109(2):163–203

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod CM, MacDonald PA (2000) Interdimensional interference in the Stroop effect: uncovering the cognitive and neural anatomy of attention. Trends Cogn Sci 4(10):383–391

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mantere T, Tupala E, Hall H, Sarkioja T, Rasanen P, Bergstrom K, Callaway J, Tiihonen J (2002) Serotonin transporter distribution and density in the cerebral cortex of alcoholic and nonalcoholic comparison subjects: a whole-hemisphere autoradiography study. Am J Psychiatry 159(4):599–606

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marksteiner J, Bodner T, Gurka P (2002) Alcohol-induced cognitive disorder: alcohol dementia. Wien Med Wochenschr 152(3–4):98–101

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Maylor EA, Rabbitt PM (1993) Alcohol, reaction time and memory: a meta-analysis. Br J Psychol 84(Pt 3):301–317

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCusker CG (2001) Cognitive biases and addiction: an evolution in theory and method. Addiction 96(1):47–56

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mega MS, Cummings JL (1994) Frontal–subcortical circuits and neuropsychiatric disorders. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 6(4):358–370

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg K, Bradley BP, Millar N, White J (1995) A follow-up study of cognitive bias in generalized anxiety disorder. Behav Res Ther 33:927–935

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Monahan JS (2001) Coloring single Stroop elements: reducing automaticity or slowing color processing? J Gen Psychol 128(1):98–112

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Moselhy HF, Georgiou G, Kahn A (2001) Frontal lobe changes in alcoholism: a review of the literature. Alcohol Alcohol 36(5):357–368

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nassauer KW, Halperin JM (2003) Dissociation of perceptual and motor inhibition processes through the use of novel computerized conflict tasks. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 9(1):25–30

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nixon SJ, Parsons OA, Schaeffer KW, Hale RL (1995) A methodological study of the Shipley Institute of Living Scale in alcoholics and non-alcoholics: reliability, discriminating items and alternate forms. Appl Neuropsychol 2(3/4):155–160

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Noel X, Van der Linden M, Schmidt N, Sferrazza R, Hanak C, Le Bon O, De Mol J, Kornreich C, Pelc I, Verbanck P (2001) Supervisory attentional system in nonamnesic alcoholic men. Arch Gen Psychiatry 58(12):1152–1158

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Noel X, Sferrazza R, Van Der Linden M, Paternot J, Verhas M, Hanak C, Pelc I, Verbanck P (2002) Contribution of frontal cerebral blood flow measured by 99 mTc-bicisate SPECT and executive function deficits to predicting treatment outcome in alcohol-dependent patients. Alcohol Alcohol 37(4):347–354

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pardo JV, Pardo PJ, Janer KW, Raichle ME (1990) The anterior cingulate cortex mediates processing selection in the Stroop attentional conflict paradigm. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 87(1):256–259

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Parker ES, Birnbaum IM, Boyd RA, Noble EP (1980) Neuropsychologic decrements as a function of alcohol intake in male students. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 4(3):330–334

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Paulus MP, Hozack N, Frank L, Brown GG (2002) Error rate and outcome predictability affect neural activation in prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate during decision-making. Neuroimage 15(4):836–846

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pothos EM, Cox WM (2002) Cognitive bias for alcohol-related information in inferential processes. Drug Alcohol Depend 66(3):235–241

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ravnkilde B, Videbech P, Rosenberg R, Gjedde A, Gade A (2002) Putative tests of frontal lobe function: a PET-study of brain activation during Stroop’s Test and verbal fluency. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 24(4):534–547

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Riemann BC, McNally RJ (1995) Cognitive processing of personally relevant information. Cogn Emot 9(4):325–340

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts AJ, Koob GF (1997) The neurobiology of addiction: an overview. Alcohol Health Res World 21(2):101–106

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson TE, Berridge KC (1993) The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Rev 18(3):247–291

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson TE, Berridge KC (2003) Addiction. Annu Rev Psychol 54(1):25–53

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salgado-Pineda P, Vendrell P, Bargallo N, Falcon C, Junque C (2002) Functional magnetic resonance in the evaluation of the activity of the anterior cingulate cortex using Stroop’s paradigm. Rev Neurol 34(7):607–611

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Selzer ML (1971) The Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test: the quest for a new diagnostic instrument. Am J Psychiatry 127(12):89–94

    Google Scholar 

  • Serra-Mestres J, Ring HA (2002) Evidence supporting a cognitive model of depression in Parkinson’s disease. J Nerv Ment Dis 190(6):407–410

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sharma D, McKenna FP (2001) The role of time pressure on the emotional Stroop task. Br J Psychol 92(Pt 3):471–481

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Skutle A, Berg G (1987) Training in controlled drinking for early-stage problem drinkers. Br J Addict 82(5):493–501

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Smith EE, Jonides J (1999) Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science 283(5408):1657–1661

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Spinks H, Dalgleish T (2001) Attentional processing and levels of symptomatology in Seasonal Affective Disorder (SAD): a preliminary longitudinal study. J Affect Disord 62(3):229–232

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stacy AW (1997) Memory activation and expectancy as prospective predictors of alcohol and marijuana use. J Abnorm Psychol 106:61–73

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Steel C, Haworth EJ, Peters E, Hemsley DR, Sharma T, Gray JA, Pickering A, Gregory L, Simmons A, Bullmore ET, Williams SC (2001) Neuroimaging correlates of negative priming. Neuroreport 12(16):3619–3624

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stetter F, Ackermann K, Bizer A, Straube ER, Mann K (1995) Effects of disease-related cues in alcoholic inpatients: results of a controlled “Alcohol Stroop” study. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 19(3):593–599

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stormark KM, Laberg JC, Nordby H, Hugdahl K (2000) Alcoholics’ selective attention to alcohol stimuli: automated processing? J Stud Alcohol 61(1):18–23

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stroop JR (1935) Studies of interference in serial verbal reaction. J Exp Psychol 18:643–662

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swick D, Jovanovic J (2002) Anterior cingulate cortex and the Stroop task: neuropsychological evidence for topographic specificity. Neuropsychologia 40(8):1240–1253

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tiffany ST (1990) A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior: role of automatic and nonautomatic processes. Psychol Rev 97(2):147–168

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • van Veen V, Cohen JD, Botvinick MM, Stenger VA, Carter CS (2001) Anterior cingulate cortex, conflict monitoring, and levels of processing. Neuroimage 14(6):1302–1308

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vogel-Sprott M, Easdon C, Fillmore M, Finn P, Justus A (2001) Alcohol and behavioral control: cognitive and neural mechanisms. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 25(1):117–121

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Vogt BA, Finch DM, Olson CR (1992) Functional heterogeneity in cingulate cortex: the anterior executive and posterior evaluative regions. Cereb Cortex 2(6):435–443

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Waters H, Green MW (2003) A demonstration of attentional bias, using a novel dual task paradigm, towards clinically salient material in recovering alcohol abuse patients? Psychol Med 33(3):491–498

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watts FN, McKenna FP, Sharrock R, Trezise L (1986) Colour naming of phobia-related words. Br J Psychol 77(Pt 1):97–108

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wehr A, Bauer LO (1999) Verbal ability predicts abstinence from drugs and alcohol in a residential treatment population. Psychol Rep 84(3 Pt 2):1354–1360

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wells A, Matthews G (1999) The cognitive science of attention and emotion. In: Dalgleish T, Power MJ (eds) Handbook of cognition and emotion. Wiley, Chichester, UK

  • Whalen PJ, Bush G, McNally RJ, Wilhelm S, McInerney SC, Jenike MA, Rauch SL (1998) The emotional counting Stroop paradigm: a functional magnetic resonance imaging probe of the anterior cingulate affective division. Biol Psychiatry 44(12):1219–1228

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wiers RW, van Woerden N, Smulders FT, de Jong PJ (2002) Implicit and explicit alcohol-related cognitions in heavy and light drinkers. J Abnorm Psychol 111(4):648–658

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiers RW, Cox WM, Field M, Fadardi JW, Palfai T, Schoenmakers T et al (2005) The search for new ways to change implicit alcohol-related cognitions in heavy drinkers. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (in press)

  • Williams JM, Mathews A, MacLeod C (1996) The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychol Bull 120(1):3–24

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wright I, Waterman M, Prescott H, Murdoch-Eaton D (2003) A new Stroop-like measure of inhibitory function development: typical developmental trends. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discipl 44(4):561–575

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zachary RA (2000) Shipley Institute of Living Scale: revised manual. Western Psychological Services, Los Angeles

    Google Scholar 

  • Zack M, Belsito L, Scher R, Eissenberg T, Corrigall WA (2001) Effects of abstinence and smoking on information processing in adolescent smokers. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 153(2):249–257

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zeigler DW, Wang CC, Yoast RA, Dickinson BD, McCaffree MA, Robinowitz CB, Sterling ML (2005) The neurocognitive effects of alcohol on adolescents and college students. Prev Med 40(1):23–32

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to W. Miles Cox.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Fadardi, J.S., Cox, W.M. Alcohol attentional bias: drinking salience or cognitive impairment?. Psychopharmacology 185, 169–178 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-005-0268-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-005-0268-0

Keywords

Navigation