Abstract
Rationale
When asked “How many animals of each kind did Moses take on the ark?”, people fail to notice the distortion introduced by the impostor “Moses” and respond “two”. It has been argued that the effect must be due to the existence of a partial-match process. In most situations, the form of a question is not likely to closely match the memory representation it queries. Thus, for the partial match hypothesis people ignore some semantic distortions. In the same vein, it has been shown that the benzodiazepine lorazepam drug induces some impairments of semantic memory as participants under lorazepam provide more incorrect recalls than placebo do with general information questions.
Objectives
The aim of this study was to investigate the effects of the benzodiazepine lorazepam on the Moses illusion paradigm.
Method
The effects of lorazepam (0.038 mg/kg) and of a placebo were investigated in 28 healthy volunteers. Twenty-two illusory questions were presented along with 72 normal general information questions.
Results
Lorazepam impaired the ability to detect the Moses illusion. Moreover, lorazepam participants appeared less biased to consider a question distorted than placebo participants.
Conclusions
The temporary and reversible semantic memory impairments experienced by participants when falling into the Moses illusion are more frequent under lorazepam. The amnesic drug lorazepam may impair semantic processing as well as the strategic control of memory.
Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
\( {\text{{A}'}} = 22/{\left( {72 + 22} \right)} + {\left( {{\left( {{\text{H}} - {\text{FA}}} \right)}{\left( {1 + {\text{H}} - {\text{FA}}} \right)}} \right)}/{\left( {4{\text{H}}{\left( {1 - {\text{FA}}} \right)}} \right)} \)
In this formula, 22 corresponded to the number of experimental Moses questions and 72 to the number of fillers.
\( {B}''d = {\left( {{\left( {1 - {\text{H}}} \right)}{\left( {1 - {\text{FA}}} \right)} - {\text{HFA}}} \right)}/{\left( {{\left( {1 - {\text{H}}} \right)}{\left( {1 - {\text{FA}}} \right)} + {\text{HFA}}} \right)}. \)
References
Allen D, Curran HV, Lader M (1993) The effects of single doses of CL 284, 846, lorazepam, and placebo on psychomotor and memory function in normal male volunteers. Eur J Clin Pharmacol 45:313–320
Bacon E, Danion JM, Kauffman-Muller F, Schelstraete MA, Bruant A, Sellal F, Grange D (1998) Confidence level and feeling of knowing for episodic and semantic memory: an investigation of lorazepam effects on metamemory. Psychopharmacology 138:318–325
Bacon E, Paire-Ficout L, Izaute M (2002) Dissociation between the subjective experience and the cognitive process: the effects of the anxiolytic drug lorazepam on TOT states. Abstr Psychon Soc 7:24
Barton SB, Sanford AJ (1993) A case study of anomaly detection: shallow semantic processing and cohesion establishment? Mem Cognit 21:477–487
Bishop K, Curran V (1998) An investigation of the effects of benzodiazepine receptor ligands and of scopolamine on conceptual priming. Psychopharmacology 140:345–353
Bond A, Lader M (1974) The use of analog scales in rating subjective feelings. Br J Med Psychol 47:211–218
Brédart S, Docquier M (1989) The Moses illusion: a follow-up on the focalization effect. Eur Bull Cognit Psychol 9:357–362
Brédart S, Modolo K (1988) Moses strikes again: Focalization effect on a semantic illusion. Acta Psychol 67:135–144
Curran HV (1999) Effects of anxiolytics on memory. Hum Psychopharmacol 14:72–79
Donaldson W (1992) Measuring recognition memory. J Exp Psychol Gen 121:275–277
Erickson TD, Mattson ME (1981) From word to meaning: a semantic illusion. J Verb Learn Verb Behav 20:540–551
File SE, Sharma R, Shaffer J (1992) Is lorazepam-induced amnesia specific to the type of memory or to the task used to assess it? J Psychopharmacol 61:76–80
Fluck E, File SE, Springett J, Kopelman MD, Rees J, Orgill J (1998) Does the sedation resulting from sleep deprivation and lorazepam cause similar cognitive deficits? Pharmacol Biochem Behav 59:909–915
Gorenstein C, Bernik MA, Pompéia S, Marcourakis T (1995). Impairment of performance associated with long-term use of benzodiazepines. J Psychopharmacol 9:313–318
Green JF, McElholm A, King DJ (1996) A comparison of the sedative and amnestic effects of chlorpromazine and lorazepam. Psychopharmacology 128:67–73
Hannon B, Daneman M (2001) Susceptibility to semantic illusions: an individual-effects differences perspective. Mem Cognit 29:449–461
Harvey SC (1980) Hypnotics and sedatives. In: Goodman Gilman A, Goodman LS, Gilman A (eds) The pharmacological basis of therapeutics (chapter 17). Macmillan, New York, pp 339–375
Huron C, Giersch A, Danion JM (2002) Lorazepam, sedation and conscious recollection: a dose-response study with healthy volunteers. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 17:19–26
Kamas EN, Reder LM, Ayers MS (1996) Partial matching in the Moses illusion: response bias not sensitivity. Mem Cognit 24:687–699
Kleven MS, Koek W (1999) Effects of benzodiazepine agonists on punished responding in pigeons and their relationship with clinical doses in humans. Psychopharmacology 141:206–212
Koriat A, Goldsmith M (1996) Memory in naturalistic and laboratory contexts: distinguish the accuracy-oriented and quantity-oriented approaches to memory assessment. J Exp Psychol Gen 123:297–315
Lories G (2002) Familiarity and the retrieval of memory traces, metacognitive aspects. In: Chambres P, Izaute M, Marescaux PJ (eds) Metacognition: process, function, and use. Kluwer, Dordrecht, pp 91–101
Massin-Krauss M, Bacon E, Danion JM (2002) Effects of the benzodiazepine lorazepam on monitoring and control processes in semantic memory. Conscious Cognit 11:123–137
Nelson TO, Narens L (1990) Metamemory: a theoretical framework and new findings. In: Bower GH (ed) The psychology of learning and motivation (vol 26). Academic Press, New York, pp 1–45
Norris H (1971) The action of sedatives on brain stem occulomotor system in man. Neuropharmacology 10:181–191
Reder LM, Cleermans A (1990) The role of partial matches in comprehension: the Moses illusion revisited. In: Graesser A, Bower GH (eds) The psychology of learning and motivation (vol 25). Academic Press, New York, pp 233–258
Reder LM, Kusbit GW (1991) Locus of the Moses illusion: Imperfect encoding, retrieval, or match? J Mem Lang 30:385–406
Schwartz BL (1998) Illusory tip-of-the-tongue states. Memory 6:623–642
Schwartz BL, Travis DM, Castro AM, Smith MS (2000) The phenomenology of real and illusory tip-of-the-tongue states. Mem Cognit 28:18–27
Shafto M, McKay DG (2000) The Moses, mega-moses, and Armstrong illusions: integrating language comprehension and semantic memory. Psychol Sci 11:372–378
Snodgrass JG, Corwin J (1988) Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: application to dementia and amnesia. J Exp Psychol Gen 117:34–50
Tata PR, Philip R, Rollings J, Collins M, Pickering A, Jacobson RR (1994) Lack of cognitive recovery following withdrawal from long-term benzodiazepine use. Psychol Med 24:203–213
van Oostendorp H, De Mul S (1990) Moses beats Adam: a semantic relatedness effect on a semantic illusion. Acta Psychol 74:35–46
van Oostendorp H, Kok I (1990) Failling to notice errors in sentences. Lang Cognit Proc 5:105–113
Vanover K E, Robledo S, Huber M, Carter RB (1999) Pharmacological evaluation of a modified conflict procedure: punished drinking in non-water-deprived rats. Psychopharmacology 145:333–341
Vermeeren A, Jackson JL, Muntjewerff ND, Quint PJ, Harrison EM, O’Hanlon JF (1995) Comparison of acute alprazolam (0.25, 0.50 and 1.0 mg) effects versus those of lorazepam 2 mg and placebo on memory in healthy volunteers using laboratory and telephone tests. Psychopharmacology 118:1–9
Wechsler D (1987) Wechsler memory scale, revisited manual. Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, Texas
Whittlesea BW (2002) False memory and the discrepancy-attribution hypothesis: the prototype-familiarity illusion. J Exp Psychol Gen 131:96–115
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by a Cognitique grant (no. COG 53B) “Perturbations and recovery of cognitive functions” from the French Ministry of Research. Laurence Paire-Ficout benefited from a post-doctoral grant from Cognitique. The authors wish to thank Christine Ramana-Keller for technical assistance, Dr. Marie Welsch for the medical examination of the participants, Markus Brauer for his advice about statistical analyses, and Professor Guy Lories and Professor Jean-Marie Danion and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on earlier versions of this article. We particularly thank Yvonne Turrell who patiently corrected our English. We also thank the INSERM and the CNRS institutions.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Izaute, M., Paire-Ficout, L. & Bacon, E. Benzodiazepines and semantic memory: effects of lorazepam on the Moses illusion. Psychopharmacology 172, 309–315 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-003-1665-x
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-003-1665-x