1 Introduction

Economists have been devoting increasing attention to the broad well-being consequences of unemployment.Footnote 1 A substantial body of literature has documented that joblessness causes large life satisfaction declines and permanently scars those directly affected (Winkelmann 2014; Clark et al. 2001). While most of the scholarship has focused on the subjective well-being consequences of one’s own unemployment, it is reasonable to expect that unemployment may affect the well-being of other family members through the negative externatilities due to sharing the same household, having a common income, and being exposed to similar stressors (Luhmann et al. 2014). Similarly, one spouse’s job loss could affect the well-being of the other spouse due to increased financial strain and worries about the future income, or due to non-monetary considerations, including the change in the amount of time spent together, social stigmatization, emotional contagion, and the spillover of negative emotions such as stress or worry (Luhmann et al. 2014).

Nevertheless, with a few exceptions, the literature on unemployment and well-being has surprisingly ignored explorations of the within-couple subjective well-being externalities. This paper fills this void by investigating how one spouse’s exogenous unemployment induced by company closings causally affects the subjective well-being of the other partner (i.e., the indirectly affected spouse).Footnote 2 Using household data from the German Socio-Economic Panel Study (GSOEP) for 1991–2015, which allow us to employ couple fixed effects, we find that spousal job loss due to workplace closure has a negative effect on the indirectly affected spouse’s life satisfaction.Footnote 3 By accounting for about one quarter to one third of the effect of one’s own unemployment on subjective well-being, the magnitude of the cross-spouse externality that we document is economically important. In particular, following his wife’s unemployment, a husband’s life satisfaction decreases by about 0.34 points (on a 0–10 scale). The decline is slightly smaller (0.25 points) for the average woman whose husband loses his job, although the gender difference is statistically insignificant. Furthermore, we also discover that the life evaluations reduction is also mirrored in the declining satisfaction with household income and living standards.

In addition, we investigate the subjective well-being trajectories of partners before and after involuntary spousal unemployment. We show that wives do not adapt to their husbands’ unemployment, while the pattern for indirectly affected husbands is less clear-cut. Our results are robust to several sensitivity tests, such as controlling for job loss expectations, panel attrition, partnership dissolution, state-specific shocks, and local-level unemployment rates. We also test for possible channels behind our results and conclude that the negative effect of spousal unemployment is independent of current and future income, the unemployment duration, as well as the couple’s position in the income distribution. Taken together with the declining satisfaction with income and living standards, these results suggest that in a country with a liberal unemployment benefits system such as Germany, the stigma attached to living on public welfare rather than the financial concerns may account for the well-being losses that we document (Chadi and Hetschko 2017).

This paper focuses on the cognitive component of subjective well-being, namely life satisfaction.Footnote 4 It thus differs from the related scholarship on the mental health consequences of spousal unemployment (e.g., Bubonya et al. 2017; Marcus 2013; Mendolia 2014; Siegel et al. 2003). At first sight, it may seem that mental health and subjective well-being are synonymous. Nonetheless, despite being conceptually related, mental health refers to the absence of depression and chronic anxiety, while life satisfaction is a global reflective assessment of one’s life as a whole (Diener et al. 1999; Flèche and Layard 2017). While both mental health and life satisfaction are subjective states, mental illness is typically diagnosed by a medical professional. Finally, the correlation coefficient between mental health and misery (low subjective well-being) is as low as 0.1 (and as high as 0.4), implying that these are two different constructs (Flèche and Layard 2017).Footnote 5

Building on the extant studies, our contribution to the literature is threefold. First, we provide a causal estimate of the effects of spousal unemployment on the other partner’s life evaluations using representative household German panel data. Second, in addition to studying unemployment's externalities in terms of spousal life satisfaction, we are also the first to consider the consequences of unemployment for the indirectly affected partner’s satisfaction with five different life domains: leisure, housework, household income, standard of living, and health. Third, we document novel results concerning the anticipation of and adaptation to spousal job loss due to plant closing and we compare and contrast these patterns with the anticipation of and adaptation to one’s own unemployment due to company closure in our sample. Finally, we carefully consider the robustness of our findings and whether income considerations could be driving our results.

We argue that a causal understanding of the size of the spillover effects of unemployment has important consequences for the design of social policy. Our findings highlight the relevance of unemployment’s within-couple externalities and expand the state-of-the-art knowledge of such interdependencies. As such, the results imply that effective public policy programs aimed at mitigating the negative consequences of unemployment should adopt a family perspective.

2 Related literature

Both cross-sectional and panel studies find unequivocal subjective well-being losses accruing to individuals who experience unemployment themselves (see Winkelmann (2014) for a review).Footnote 6 Specifically, past unemployment experiences reduce current subjective well-being levels conditional on current labor market status (Clark et al. 2001). Even job loss expectations can be as devastating as the unemployment experience itself (Witte 1999). In addition, individuals do not adapt to unemployment in virtually every context in which this relationship has been investigated: even five years after becoming unemployment, men’s life satisfaction scores continue to decline. The results for women are not so clear-cut (Clark 2016).Footnote 7

Using the only cross-section from the British Household Panel Study (BHPS) available at the time, Clark and Oswald (1994) were among the first to document the negative association between personal unemployment and subjective well-being. Furthermore, Gerlach and Stephan (1996) and Winkelmann and Winkelmann (1998) furnish the first panel regression estimates demonstrating the negative life satisfaction consequences of unemployment in Germany, which amount to about one point on a 0–10 scale. The pecuniary costs of unemployment (i.e., the loss of income) are about two to seven times smaller compared to the non-monetary costs related to loss of status and work identity (Winkelmann and Winkelmann 1998; Knabe and Rätzel 2011a, b). Given the large non-pecuniary costs of job loss, the early literature concluded (yet did not formally test) that unemployment entry is largely involuntary. Indeed, as the first to distinguish between voluntary and involuntary job loss, Kassenboehmer and Haisken-DeNew (2009) find that company closures—which proxy unexpected and involuntary job loss—lead to individual life satisfaction declines of about 0.5 points on a 0–10 scale.

Nevertheless, to date no study has examined whether and how one spouse’s unemployment causally affects the other partner’s subjective well-being.Footnote 8 One related early contribution in the economics literature (Winkelmann and Winkelmann 1995) has studied cross-spousal spillovers in life satisfaction due to unemployment. Specifically, using GSOEP data for West Germany for 1984–1989, Winkelmann and Winkelmann (1995) show one regression table whereby in addition to controlling for one’s own unemployment status, the authors also include indicators for the partner’s employment status.Footnote 9 The study shows that conditional on one’s own unemployment, partners’ unemployment reduces female life satisfaction by 0.5 points (on a 0–10 scale). Men do not experience additional declines in life satisfaction above and beyond their own unemployment. While the Winkelmann and Winkelmann (1995) work is important in highlighting the within-couple externalities of unemployment, it relies on data only from West Germany from the late-1980s and does not differentiate between voluntary and involuntary unemployment entry. Specifically, own life satisfaction or the life satisfaction of the spouse may be correlated with the unemployment entry, thus biasing upwards the coefficient estimates in Winkelmann and Winkelmann (1995).

We also point out that our paper substantively differs from Luhmann et al. (2014), who study 2973 couples in the GSOEP during the years 1984–2009 and document their life satisfaction trajectories before and after unemployment using research methods from the psychology discipline. The models that the authors use have a number of limitations. For instance, they do not have couple or individual fixed effects and as such cannot rule out individual or couple-level unobserved heterogeneity. Moreover, the paper only includes a limited number of covariates (labor status, job security worry, children, relationship duration, income gender, and age), and individuals appear in the regressions more than once as part of the same couple. The associated declines in partners’ well-being is about 0.32 points on a 0–10 scale (relative to the baseline) and the authors document anticipation effects one year before spousal unemployment and no adaptation two years following the unemployment episode.

Finally, the present study is related to but distinct from the growing body of the literature on the mental health consequences of spousal unemployment. One of the first nationally representative analyses offering a household-level perspective on unemployment uses 1987 Irish data to demonstrate that husbands’ unemployment is unassociated with wives’ mental health (Whelan 1994). Exploiting British panel data, Clark (2003) discovers that the negative mental well-being consequences of unemployment are partially mitigated if the spouse or other household members are also unemployed, highlighting the social norm of unemployment. Using panel data on older adults from the Health and Retirement Study, Siegel et al. (2003) find no association between husbands’ job loss and wives’ mental health, which is consistent with the cross-sectional results of Whelan (1994). Several papers explicitly tackle the endogeneity between spousal job loss and the other partner’s mental health outcomes. Specifically, based on 2002–2010 German panel data and a difference-in-differences matching estimator, Marcus (2013) finds that unemployment negatively affects own mental health as well as the mental health of the other spouse, with the latter effect being smaller. His results further suggest that the decreases in spousal mental health are larger when the husband (rather than the wife) becomes jobless. Mendolia (2014) tackles the endogeneity of job loss by focusing on redundancies in declining industries. Using a logit fixed effects model and the first 14 waves of BHPS, she finds that both partners in British couples in which the man loses his job experience poor mental health. Bubonya et al. (2017) utilize Australian panel data and mitigate reverse causality problems by relying on a fixed effects analysis in combination with lagged spousal unemployment. They demonstrate that husbands’ mental health does not deteriorate due to their wives’ job loss. However, the mental well-being of wives declines after their husbands’ unemployment, but only if the job loss episode is sustained, or couple had financial difficulties or relationship strain prior to the unemployment.

3 Data and variables

3.1 Data

We rely on the GSOEP, which is a representative household panel in Germany (version 32.1) (Frick et al. 2007; SOEP 2016). The data offer a rich set of variables related to subjective well-being, health, demographic and labor market characteristics, income, household composition and finances, as well as family biography.

The main dependent variable is overall life satisfaction measured on a scale of 0 (completely dissatisfied) to 10 (completely satisfied). In separate regressions, we utilize variables capturing satisfaction with different life domains such as leisure time, housework, income, standard of living, and health status, all of which are measured on a 0–10 scale (see Table 10 in Appendix A for variable definitions). The satisfaction with housework is only asked to those respondents who are engaged in household chores.

The GSOEP includes a variable reporting all types of labor force status since the last interview, including private employment, civil service, self-employment, apprenticeship/traineeship, registered unemployment, retirement, and being in education. In addition—and importantly for our identification strategy—starting in 1985, the GSOEP introduced a question on the reasons for job termination and since 1991 the answers have included a category for “place of work closed,” which allows distinguishing between voluntary and involuntary unemployment (Kassenboehmer and Haisken-DeNew 2009). We exploit this information to construct an indicator for exogenous spousal unemployment. However, since the possible answer categories in 1999 and 2000 excluded company closure, we omit these two years from our analyses.

Our analysis also utilizes the information on indirectly affected respondents’ demographic and labor market characteristics such as age, education, health (number of doctor visits and disability status), marital and labor force status, working hours, as well as household characteristics such as number of children, household size, household income, home ownership, and place of residence. The year 1993 is excluded from the estimations due to the unavailability of the doctor visits variable. In separate regressions, we also include controls for the directly affected spouse’s future job loss expectations and the duration of the unemployment episode due to plant closing. Table 10 in Appendix A describes the variables used in the analysis.

3.2 Analysis sample

Our analysis samples comprise cohabiting couples—regardless of their formal marital status—matched based on spouse and household identifier variables observed from 1991 to 2015, with the exception of the years 1993, 1999, and 2000. We separately examine couples in which the husband enters unemployment from those in which the wife enters unemployment. The directly affected spouses are defined as those who potentially experience unemployment due to plant closure and are full- or part-time private employees in the previous time period (time t − 1). The “private employee” category excludes pensioners, civil servants, self-employed, and those in military/community service and students.Footnote 10 In the current time period (time t), directly affected spouses either continue to be private employees or become registered unemployed and experience unemployment due to company closure.Footnote 11 In other words, spouses in our treated and comparison groups start from the same initial labor market condition, but in the next period we observe them, some experience joblessness due to company closures (treated group) while others remain continuously employed (comparison group). To capture the working-age population, we restrict the ages of the directly affected spouses to range between 18 and 67, while the indirectly affected spouses can be of any age and employment status. We exclude couples in which both the husband and wife simultaneously experienced job loss due to plant closure.

Our “husband” analysis sample (where the wife potentially experiences exogenous job loss) comprises 51,816 couple-years, or 10,682 couples, among which 201 wives experience job loss due to plant closure. The “wife” sample (where the husband potentially experiences exogenous job loss) includes 62,903 couple-years, or 12,208 couples, among which 280 husbands lose their jobs due to workplace closures. Tables 11 and 12 in the Appendix furnish summary statistics.

4 Empirical specification

The life satisfaction Y of the indirectly affected spouse i observed at time period t can be specified as:

$$ {Y}_{it}=\alpha +\beta {U^S}_{it}+X{\prime}_{it}\varOmega +{\gamma}_i+{\tau}_t+{\varepsilon}_{it} $$
(1)

In this setup, the unit of observation is the couple-year, while US is a binary indicator for spousal job loss due to workplace closure. In the “wife” sample, US takes the value of 1 if the husband changes his labor force status from being a private employee to being registered unemployed due to a plant closure, and 0 if he remains in private employment. Similarly, in the “husband” sample, US is defined with respect to the wife’s employment status. Furthermore, X is a vector denoting the indirectly affected spouse’s individual-level characteristics (age, age squared, years of schooling, labor force status, annual work hours, annual doctor visits, and disability status) as well as a vector of household-level controls (including the number of children, household size, home ownership, (log of) total disposable household income, indicator for west/east of Germany and federal state), γ and τ are individual and year fixed effects, respectively, and ε is the stochastic error term. Given the individual fixed effects, the subjective well-being consequences of spousal unemployment are identified from variation coming from couples in which one partner changes their labor force status from being a private employee to being registered unemployed due to a plant closure. Furthermore, as explained in Section 3.2, by construction, spouses are matched using the spouse and household identifier variables. Each individual is thus representative of the couple and individual fixed effects are thus also couple fixed effects. As such, the fixed effects capture selection issues associated with household formation such as assortative mating. Our specifications are therefore robust to certain types of endogeneity related to time-invariant individual- and couple-level heterogeneity that is simultaneously correlated with job loss and subjective well-being. All models are estimated separately for the indirectly affected husband’s and wife’s life satisfaction outcomes, and include robust standard errors, clustered at the individual (couple) level.

One objection to the specification above is that the income variable may be endogenous with spousal employment status: unemployment reduces household income, which in turn lowers life satisfaction. Ideally, we would like to control for an income source that is unaffected by spousal unemployment, although finding such an income variable is non-trivial. To understand the extent to which our results could be driven by the endogeneity of the income variable, we compare and contrast specifications that control for household income from asset flows (savings, dividends, and rents) with those that include disposable household income. While also potentially being endogenous, household income from asset flows is only indirectly related to labor income. In addition, we also offer specifications that exclude the income variable. Nevertheless, excluding income from the regressions altogether changes the interpretation of the US coefficient estimate. Specifically, when we control for household income, the parameter β represents the non-pecuniary “costs” of spousal unemployment, i.e., the well-being consequences of unemployment above and beyond income (Winkelmann and Winkelmann 1998). Given the similarity of the results with both income variables, our preferred specification is the one controlling for real disposable household income, which is conventional in the literature on subjective well-being and unemployment.

Because life satisfaction is an ordinal variable measured on 0–10 scale, we should technically estimate Eq. (1) using an ordinal logit or probit regression. As is common in the literature, all specifications use ordinary least squares (OLS) individual fixed effects since ignoring the ordinality of the data holds little consequence for the end result (Ferrer-i-Carbonell and Frijters 2004). The fixed effects regressions and the fact that unemployment entry due to company closure is exogenous to spousal life satisfaction allow for the causal interpretation of our estimates. Nevertheless, one may be concerned about the violation of the assumption on the exogeneity of unemployment due to plant closure if employees or their spouses anticipate unemployment. We test the validity of this assumption by including a control variable for the (lagged) job loss expectations of the directly affected spouse. In addition, we provide a battery of sensitivity checks such as controlling for attrition and couple dissolution, accounting for the duration of the current unemployment episode, controlling for state-specific macroeconomic shocks, regional-level unemployment shocks, weighting, and using voluntary unemployment entry. We also show specifications using unemployment due to dismissals, which is another source of involuntary job loss but is less exogenous than plant closures.

Finally, we explore the extent to which our results are driven by income considerations and provide novel results regarding the anticipation of and adaptation to spousal unemployment following a method used in Clark and Georgellis (2013) and Qari (2014). Specifically, we model anticipation and adaptation as:

$$ {Y}_{it}=\alpha +{\beta}_{-3}{U^S}_{-3, it}+{\beta}_{-2}{U^S}_{-2, it}+{\beta}_{-1}{U^S}_{-1, it}+{\beta}_0{U^S}_{0, it}+{\beta}_1{U^S}_{1, it}+{\beta}_2{U^S}_{2, it}+X{\prime}_{it}\varOmega +{\gamma}_i+{\tau}_t+{\varepsilon}_{it} $$
(2)

where the life satisfaction (Y) of the indirectly affected spouse, the vector of covariates (X), and γ and τ are individual and year fixed effects, respectively, as in Eq. (1). Importantly, Eq. (2) estimates the anticipation and adaptation effects based on the treated sample, i.e., all couples who will eventually experience unemployment due to plant clsoings.Footnote 12 To capture the anticipation of spousal unemployment, we introduce the leads of the plant closing indicator in Eq. (2). The three dummies US1, US−2, and US−3 refer to future entry into unemployment and are coded as 1 if the respondent’s spouse will enter unemployment due to plant closure in the next 1–2 years, 2–3 years, and 3 years or more, respectively and 0 otherwise. Similarly, US0 denotes the entry to unemployment due to plant closure in the first year of unemployment. To capture the adaptation to spousal unemployment, we introduce the lags of the plant closing indicator in Eq. (2). The two dummy variables US1 and US2 take the value of 1 if the respondent has been unemployed due to plant closure for 1–2 years and more than 2 years, respectively, and 0 otherwise. US−3 is the reference category in the estimation of Eq. (2). Given that all unemployment dummies are mutually exclusive, any individual is only present in one of the six categories at any given year. As we estimate (2) using fixed effects models, the comparison is within-individual and with respect to the omitted category US−3. Thus, for example, the coefficient estimate of US1 should be interpreted as the change in the well-being of indirectly affected spouses whose partners have been unemployed for 1–2 years compared to the life satisfaction scores of the same individuals 3 (or more) years before their partners enter unemployment. Similarly, the coefficient estimate on the lead variable US−1 is interpreted as the life satisfaction response of the spouse whose partner will lose her/his job due to plant closure in the next 1 to 2 years.

5 Main results

Table 1 presents the well-being consequences of involuntary spousal unemployment due to company closures for the indirectly affected spouses.Footnote 13 All regressions include individual (i.e., couple) fixed effects and controls for the year of survey and state of residence. The left panel of the table (columns (1)–(3)) reports the results for men whose (female) partners experience plant closures. Similarly, the right panel (columns (4)–(6)) reports the women’s life satisfaction response to their (male) partners’ job loss. Models (1) and (4) do not control for income, Models (2) and (5) include a variable for (the log of) real household income from asset flows, while Models (3) and (6) control for (the log of) real disposable household income. In subsequent regressions, we only show the results controlling for disposable household income.

Table 1 Effect of one spouse’s job loss on the life satisfaction of the other spouse, fixed effects

The estimates presented in Table 1 unequivocally demonstrate that regardless of gender, spousal job loss due to company closure negatively influences the life satisfaction of the indirectly affected partner. Men’s life satisfaction declines by about 0.34 points as a result of their wives’ unemployment. The effect is slightly less pronounced among women whose spouses become jobless, amounting to a reduction in perceived well-being by about 0.25 points. However, the gender difference is not statistically significant.Footnote 14

These coefficient estimates differ from those in Winkelmann and Winkelmann (1995), who find that—controlling for one’s own and one’s partner’s employment status—indirectly affected wives experience a 0.5-point drop in life satisfaction, while the coefficient estimate for males whose wives become jobless is positive and statistically insignificant. The divergence of our results from Winkelmann and Winkelmann (1995) could be due to the different sample selection and analysis period. More importantly, unlike in Winkelmann and Winkelmann (1995), our estimates are based on an exogenous unemployment entry. Comparing the coefficient estimates for indirectly affected women that we find (0.25) with those in Winkelmann and Winkelmann (0.5) implies an upward bias when selection is not taken into account.

Furthermore, the results remain unchanged when excluding income and controlling for household income from asset flows, which suggests that our results capture the non-pecuniary costs of spousal unemployment. Benchmarked against the coefficient estimates of one’s own unemployment due to firm closure for the couple sample (Table 14 in Appendix B), our results represent about one third to one quarter of the life satisfaction effects of one’s own unemployment.

Another way to quantify how much spouses care about their partners’ job loss is to utilize the life satisfaction valuation approach. Specifically, we calculate the amount of household income required to compensate the indirectly affected spouse for the life satisfaction penalty resulting from their partner’s job loss. Based on the sample mean of real household income of 46,000 Euro, male partners need to be given about 49,300 Euro. For indirectly affected women, the corresponding compensation is about 52,000 Euro.Footnote 15 The findings thus far indicate that the costs of unemployment are non-monetary and large and are also borne by indirectly affected spouses.

The coefficient estimates of the control variables in Table 1 are generally in line with the economic theory and previous studies. For instance, a higher educational attainment is positively correlated with the indirectly affected spouse’s own life satisfaction. Likewise, home ownership—which is a proxy for wealth—and household income are positively associated with spousal life satisfaction. Like other studies using the GSOEP (Frijters and Beatton 2012; Kassenboehmer and Haisken-DeNew 2012), we fail to document a U-shaped relationship between life satisfaction and age in fixed effects estimations (see Ulloa et al. 2013 for a review). Finally, disability status and the annual doctor visits—both of which are proxies for health status—are negatively associated with life satisfaction. In addition, while the magnitudes of the coefficient estimates for the control variables are generally the same across genders, those for one’s own unemployment have a substantially larger negative effect on men compared with women (based on the coefficient estimate for registered unemployed).

6 Robustness checks

In this section, we explore whether our results are robust to a battery of sensitivity tests. Our first check concerns the exogeneity of the company closure variable. Like Kassenboehmer and Haisken-DeNew (2009), we argue that plant closures are exogenous from the perspective of the individual respondent or their spouse. Nevertheless, one concern might be that employees who remain at the firm until the very end may have had reasons for doing so despite knowing about the looming closure. As Kassenboehmer and Haisken-DeNew (2009) point out, if a company takes a few months or years to shut down, then only the least flexible employees would remain until the end, thus implying that the estimated effects represent an upper bound of the actual average effect of spousal unemployment on life satisfaction. To ensure that the plant closure experience was indeed exogenous, Table 2 furnishes results controlling for the lagged directly affected spouse’s unemployment expectations (see Table 10 in Appendix A for variable definitions). In separate specifications (columns (2) and (4)), we also code all missing values for this variable as “no response” and include them in the regressions. This coding helps to avoid losing observations when we construct the lagged variable of job loss expectation and because this question was asked every other year. Specifically, the sample size declines by more than half for the husband sample and about two thirds in the wife sample (columns (1) and (3)), which explains the slightly higher coefficient estimates that the spousal job loss variable attracts in these regressions. Nevertheless, the coefficient estimates of the key regressor reported in Table 2 ((columns (2) and (4)) are substantively similar to those in Table 1, suggesting that the plant closing variable can be treated as exogenous.

Table 2 Effect of one spouse’s job loss on the life satisfaction of the other spouse, with a control for job loss expectation, fixed effects

Second, given the panel structure of our data, attrition—or the non-random exit of spouses from the dataset for reasons correlated with the unemployment episode—is a potential concern. To understand whether it biases the estimates, following Wooldridge’s (2002) approach we constructed a binary indicator taking the value of one if the indirectly affected spouse disappears from the sample in the next time period and zero if he/she stays in the panel. In Table 3, Models (1) and (3), we show that controlling for sample attrition does not influence the results. The interaction term between the attrition dummy and the spousal job loss variable is also statistically insignificant for both genders, thus demonstrating that the cross-partner spillovers that we identify are not disproportionally higher or lower for survey dropouts.Footnote 16,Footnote 17

Table 3 Effect of one spouse’s job loss on the life satisfaction of the other spouse, with controls for attrition and divorce, fixed effects

A related issue arises from the fact that partners may drop from the sample due to divorce or separation, which could be partly induced by the job loss episode (Doiron and Mendolia 2012; Eliason 2012; Hansen 2005; Rege et al. 2007). By construction, our sample comprises cohabiting couples, which may be a self-selected sample of all couples experiencing unemployment. Given that the plant closing episode happened at any time since the past interview, our partner sample may represent those with strong marriage ties or other unobserved reasons to remain together. We test whether partnership dissolution through divorce and separation following the unemployment episode influences the results. Specifically, exploiting the panel structure of the data, for the couples for which we have marital status information in the next time period, we created a binary indicator variable that is equal to one if the indirectly affected spouse separates or divorces from their current partner in the subsequent observation period. The findings reported in Table 3, columns (2) and (4), indicate that the main effect of spousal unemployment remains robust to this check. In addition, being divorced or separated in the next time period and the interaction term between divorce and spousal job loss is statistically insignificant for both the husband and wife sub-samples. This suggests that divorce among couples experiencing unemployment does not bias the results. Yet, these results should be interpreted with caution as only 6 out of 188 treated couples experience subsequent separation in column (2) and only 15 out of 264 couples do so in column (4).Footnote 18

A third identification threat may arise if plant closures are the result of certain regional shocks that also simultaneously influence life satisfaction. For example, firm closures may be driven by an overall decline in the regional economy and affect individual life satisfaction, which would upwardly bias our results. To understand the extent to which such region-specific changes matter, we conducted the analyses by including additional year×federal state interaction terms. Table 4 shows that the results in this specification remain very similar to the main results presented in Table 1, although the coefficient estimates for the spousal unemployment variable are slightly lower compared to those in the main specification in the male sample.Footnote 19 In summary, Tables 2, 3, and 4 imply that the finding that unexpected and involuntary spousal unemployment leads to cross-spouse life satisfaction declines is robust across different specifications and sensitivity tests.Footnote 20,Footnote 21

Table 4 Effect of one spouse’s job loss on the life satisfaction of the other spouse, controlling for state-specific shocks, fixed effects

7 Channels and explanations

7.1 The income channel revisited

Our results thus far suggest that the cross-spouse declines in life satisfaction associated with partner unemployment are largely independent of current income. However, it is possible that the psychological costs of unemployment borne by indirectly affected spouses are due to financial concerns about the future. As Chadi and Hetschko (2017) note, income losses resulting from unemployment may be unimportant for subjective well-being as the unemployed substitute their former labor income with the income of other household members and unemployment benefits. Furthermore, Germany provides a relatively substantial unemployment insurance package in the first year of unemployment, which may mitigate the associated labor income loss. However, as the unemployment duration increases, the unemployment benefits, the re-employment probability, and household income decrease. Thus, the well-being loss resulting from spousal unemployment may reflect concerns about the directly affected partner’s future labor market prospects and earnings rather than worries about the current situation.

To understand the extent to which such concerns about the future influence our results, in Table 5 we control for the duration (in months) of the spouse’s current unemployment episode (Models (1) and (3)) and household income in the next survey period (Models (2) and (4)).Footnote 22 The duration of the unemployment episode of the partner is a count variable coded as 0 if the respondent’s spouse is currently employed. The main results and conclusions remain virtually unaltered. The coefficient estimates for the duration of the current unemployment episode is relatively small in magnitude and statistically insignificant. Furthermore, Models (2) and (4) show that while future household income—which captures financial concerns about the future—is positively associated with current life satisfaction, controlling for it does not seem to eliminate the magnitude of the spousal job loss coefficient, thus suggesting that the results are not driven by considerations about future re-employment or finances.

Table 5 Effect of one spouse’s job loss on the life satisfaction of the other spouse, controls for unemployment duration and future household income, fixed effects

In Table 6, we further explore whether the results depend on who the couple’s primary earner is.Footnote 23 The intuition is that spousal unemployment may not be as detrimental for the indirectly affected spouse if the unemployed partner was not the primary “breadwinner” in the couple. Nonetheless, the results in columns (1) and (6) demonstrate that the primary earner variable and its interaction with spousal job loss indicator are statistically insignificant, suggesting that the main results are independent of this factor.

Table 6 Effect of one spouse’s job loss on the life satisfaction of the other spouse, with interactions for primary earner and income quartiles, fixed effects

Next, we explore the extent to which the main effects that we identify differ for couples at the bottom and top of the household income distribution. For this purpose, in separate regressions, we interact an indicator variable for bottom, middle, and top quartile earners with the job loss variable. The results presented in columns (2)–(5) of Table 6, respectively, demonstrate that there are no differential effects at any quartile of the earnings distribution for indirectly affected husbands. In other words, spousal job loss equally hits indirectly affected spouses coming from both relatively poor and relatively rich households. The interaction term of the top income quartile is negatively and marginally statistically significant for indirectly affected wives (column (10)), implying that women in relatively well-off households may be disproportionately negatively affected by their partner’s joblessness. This result may be driven by the stronger social norm to be successful among higher-status households.Footnote 24

The results presented in this section imply that the life satisfaction penalty from involuntary spousal unemployment is generally not due to pecuniary factors; rather, it likely reflects the psychological distress and other non-pecuniary costs of unemployment such as social stigma.Footnote 25

7.2 Is there a social norm of unemployment at the couple level?

Following Clark (2003), we test whether there is a social norm of unemployment within the couple, i.e., whether one’s own unemployment mitigates the negative well-being consequences of spousal job loss due to plant closure. Using a cross-section of respondents from Berlin and Magdeburg in 2008, Knabe et al. (2016) examine how one’s own unemployment depends on the labor force status of the partner. The authors find that the large well-being gap between unemployed and employed partnered men (4.3 points on a 0–10 scale) is mitigated if their spouses are unemployed or inactive. Among partnered women, one’s own unemployment is associated with a 1.1 point decline in life satisfaction, which is partially mitigated by having an inactive partner. However, our results do not support the evidence of a social norm of unemployment at the couple level. The results in Table 7 document that while personal unemployment is negatively associated with life satisfaction, the interaction term is statistically insignificant, suggesting that one’s own unemployment has no additional effect above and beyond the negative impact of spousal job loss due to company closure. Nonetheless, one should be cautious in interpreting these results given the lack of sufficient statistical power to identify the effects.

Table 7 Effect of one spouse’s job loss on the life satisfaction of the other spouse, social norms of unemployment, fixed effects

7.3 Satisfaction with life domains

In Table 8, we further explore whether and how spousal unemployment affects satisfaction with housework, free time, household income, living standards, and health. While Chadi and Hetschko (2017) study satisfaction with different life domains resulting from individual unemployment, to our knowledge no comparable analyses exist at the couple level. Like the life evaluations variable, the regressands in Table 8 are subjective assessments of the different life aspects measured on a 0–10 scale.

Table 8 The effects of one spouse's job loss on the satisfaction with different life domains of the other spouse, fixed effects

We provide some intuition about the expected results. First, concerning the regressions using satisfaction with housework as the dependent variable, the gender identity hypothesis (Akerlof and Kranton 2000) posits that individuals derive “identity utility” from adhering to social norms. Thus, upon becoming unemployed, men—traditionally viewed as “breadwinners”—may shy away from housework, while unemployed women may substitute market work with household work (Grogan and Koka 2013). Therefore, we expect that satisfaction with household work would increase for indirectly affected men (whose wives enter unemployment) but remain unchanged or decrease for indirectly affected women.Footnote 26 Second, if spouses have a complementarity in leisure and value time spend together more than income or social status, then satisfaction with free time for the indirectly affected spouse could increase following spousal job loss. This expectation is also inspired by the finding in Chadi and Hetschko (2017) that satisfaction with free time and family life increases for those who themselves become unemployed. Third, given that the consequences of unemployment are largely non-pecuniary and that unemployment benefits in Germany are relatively high in the first year of unemployment, we expected no effects of spousal job loss on satisfaction with household income and satisfaction with living standards unless the satisfaction comes from the subjective valuation of labor income. Finally, given the finding in Marcus (2013) that spousal unemployment affects the indirectly affected partner’s mental health, we anticipated that spousal unemployment attracts negative coefficient estimates for both genders in the health satisfaction regressions.

Consistent with our expectations, Table 8 shows that while spousal involuntary unemployment is negatively associated with the other partner’s health satisfaction, the relationship is positive between unemployment and housework satisfaction, although both coefficient estimates are statistically insignificant. The null impact of spousal job loss on health satisfaction is in line with Schmitz (2011), who shows that plant closings do not affect individual health satisfaction. While unemployed individuals increase their time spent in housework (Chadi and Hetschko 2017), this increase in home production does not translate into increased housework satisfaction among their partners.

In addition, as noted above, spousal unemployment could be positively associated with leisure satisfaction if indirectly affected partners value their time spent together more than household income or social status associated with employment. Despite the statistical insignificance, the negative sign of the coefficient estimate on unemployment in the leisure satisfaction regressions in the male sample (Model (2) in Table 8) implies that the loss of income and the social status concerns dominate the leisure complementarity between spouses. The opposite conclusion holds true in the female sample (Model (7) in Table 8), again despite the statistical insignificance of the spousal unemployment variable.

Nevertheless, the main insight from Table 8 is that spousal job loss due to firm closure strongly reduces the other partner’s satisfaction with household income and satisfaction with living standards. Indirectly affected men’s income satisfaction declines by 0.7 points, which is enormous compared to the corresponding overall life satisfaction loss triggered by spousal unemployment. The corresponding losses for indirectly affected wives are even greater, being about one point on a 0–10 scale. These large coefficient estimates at the couple level are similarly documented at the individual level by Chadi and Hetschko (2017), who report income satisfaction declines of about 1.6 points (on a 0–10 scale) following one’s own unemployment. Table 8 also demonstrates that satisfaction with living standards declines by about 0.4 points for male partners and by 0.3 points for female partners.

These results suggest that both genders experience a deterioration in their household’s perceived financial well-being, but not in terms of other life aspects. Upon first glance, this finding may appear at odds with our result that income does not explain the life satisfaction loss associated with spousal joblessness. Our explanation hinges on modifying the argument in Chadi and Hetschko (2017), who explain for the negative income satisfaction coefficient associated with individual unemployment with the disutility of not earning labor income. Specifically, couples in which one spouse is unemployed likely cover their consumption needs with the earnings of the other spouse or via income from public transfers. Thus, while the cross-spouse psychological costs of unemployment are independent of income, the large income satisfaction declines shown in Table 8 could be due to the loss of spousal labor income and the stigma effects associated with unemployment and living on public welfare (Chadi and Hetschko 2017). Thus, the negative coefficient estimates for these perceived financial well-being variables likely reflect the disutility of having a partner who fails to comply with the social norm of working and earning a living (Stutzer & Lalive 2004). This reasoning further explains the larger negative coefficient estimate for women whose husbands become unemployed, highlighting the social norm of husbands as the breadwinners.

In subsequent analyses, we investigate whether the decline in satisfaction with income and standard of living is driven by an income effect. Specifically, in Table 9 we present results whereby in separate regressions we exclude the household income variable and control for household asset income as well as income in the next time period. Evidently, for both genders and when satisfaction with both household income and living standards are the dependent variables, the coefficient estimates on spousal unemployment do not differ much depending on whether we include or exclude income or whether we control for household asset income or future income. These results are in line with the findings in Table 1 and suggest that the actual financial conditions (current and future) do not drive our effects; rather, the well-being consequences of unemployment that we document are likely attributable to the worsened perception about the financial situation and the subsequent psychological burden imposed on the partners of the unemployed.

Table 9 The effects of one spouse's job loss on the other spouse's satisfaction with household income and satisfaction with living standards, with and without income controls, fixed effects

7.4 Anticipation of and adaptation to spousal unemployment

Earlier work provides strong empirical evidence of both lead and lagged effects of one’s own unemployment on current life satisfaction. According to Clark and Georgellis (2013), while life satisfaction normalizes (although it does not necessarily return to baseline) after some life events, there is no adaptation to unemployment among males. To evaluate the degree of anticipation and adaptation to spousal involuntary unemployment, we estimate the model described in Eq. (2) using the leads and lags of the unemployment indicator. As explained in Section 3, the estimation of Eq. (2) is based on the treated sample in which directly affected spouses lose their jobs due to plant closure. Therefore, one should be cautious in comparing the results with those from our main specifications (i.e., Tables 19) which rely on a much larger sample (namely, including both treated and comparison couples).

Figure 1 illustrates the spousal life satisfaction patterns before and after the other partner becomes unemployed due to plant closure. The left panel of the graph refers to the “husband sample” (whereby the female spouse becomes unemployed) and the right panel illustrates the results from the “wife sample” (whereby the male partner experiences a plant closure). The results suggest strong lead and lagged effects of unemployment for female partners. Importantly, indirectly affected wives’ life satisfaction does not bounce back to the baseline score two years after husbands’ unemployment, suggesting that there is no adaptation to unemployment for this group. The picture is less clear-cut for male partners whose wives lose their jobs due to plant closures. The confidence intervals are notably larger for the “husband sample” due to the smaller sample size within the observation period (compared with the “wife sample”).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Source: authors based on GSOEP 1991–2015. Notes: the figure shows the coefficient estimates of the lead and lagged unemployment dummies from the estimation of Eq. (2). The left panel refers to the “husband sample” (whereby the female spouse gets unemployed) and the right panel illustrates the results for the “wife sample” (whereby the male partner experiences a plant closure). While the dashed lines refer to the 95% confidence interval, the solid line denotes the coefficient estimates of {US−2, US−1, US0, US1, US2} specified in Eq. (2). Accordingly, the x-axis on the range of [−2,2] refers to the number of years before and after the partner gets unemployed, and the vertical solid line (crossing 0) illustrates the time of entry into unemployment. The y-axis denotes the life satisfaction outcome. The reference category is US−3. Given the fixed effects estimation, the coefficient estimates should be interpreted as the change in life satisfaction (within a person) with respect to the score three (or more) years before the partner gets unemployed due to plant closure

For comparison purposes, Fig. 2 still relies on the couple sample but shows the results of one’s own life satisfaction patterns before and after involuntary unemployment over the same period. The results are in line with those documented by Clark and Georgellis (2013). When men experience job loss, we find evidence of no adaptation of life satisfaction to their own unemployment, whereas the picture is again not so clear-cut when women become jobless. The imprecise estimates in the cases when women become jobless in Figs. 1 and 2 are attributed to the small number of “treated” observations (i.e., fewer women than men lose their jobs due to plant closures, see also Table 11 in Appendix A). Thus, we show the female sample results for illustrative purposes but refrain from drawing solid conclusions about anticipation and adaptation patterns.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Source: authors based on GSOEP 1991–2015. Notes: the figure shows the coefficient estimates of the lead and lagged unemployment dummies from the estimation of Eq. (2) for directly affected individuals. The left panel refers to cohabiting husbands who themselves get unemployed and the right panel illustrates the results for wives who themselves experience plant closure. While the dashed lines refer to the 95% confidence interval, the solid line denotes the coefficient estimates of {US−2, US−1, US0, US1, US2} specified in Eq. (2). Accordingly, the x-axis on the range of [−2,2] refers to the number of years before and after unemployment, and the vertical solid line (crossing 0) illustrates the time of entry into unemployment. The y-axis denotes the life satisfaction outcome. The reference category is US−3. Given the fixed effects estimation, the coefficient estimates should be interpreted as the change in life satisfaction (within a person) with respect to the score three (or more) years before the entering unemployment due to plant closure

8 Conclusion

Personal unemployment has large psychological costs that extend beyond the mere income loss. In this paper, we use representative German household panel data with information on exogenous unemployment entry to provide causal evidence that the non-pecuniary costs of unemployment are also borne by spouses. Husbands’ life satisfaction drops by about 0.34 points following their spouse’s unemployment, while the life satisfaction of wives whose partner becomes unemployed drops by about 0.25 points (on a 0–10 scale). The suffering imposed on the partners is large and would require compensations of about 49,000 Euros for indirectly affected males and about 52,000 Euros for indirectly affected females to bring spouses to their previous well-being levels. While men largely appear to adapt to their wives’ unemployment, women whose husbands experienced plant closures suffer life satisfaction declines even three years after unemployment. Our results are robust to a number of sensitivity checks, including controlling for the job loss anticipation, accounting for panel attrition, couple dissolution, and regional shocks.

As in the case of individual unemployment, the estimated effects are independent of income but result in large declines in perceived financial situation. Similar to Chadi and Hetschko (2017), we interpret the large dissatisfaction with income and living standards following spousal unemployment as a marker of the high value attached to market work, the disutility of living on public assistance, and accordingly, the social stigma of being jobless. Overall, these findings imply that the large non-pecuniary costs of spousal unemployment are due to psychological scarring from the unemployment experience itself.

Our analysis has several policy implications. First, policies aimed at alleviating the consequences of unemployment should also consider other household members and especially female partners. Second, in the spirit of Bubonya et al. (2017), we argue that unemployment policies that simply provide monetary assistance are unlikely to restore affected families’ well-being. At the same time, policies that seek to reduce the psychological burden related to the stigma of being unemployed or receiving unemployment benefits could be a double-edged sword if they prolong the unemployment duration. Finally, our findings are also informative to employers who may want to consider a household perspective when designing severance packages. They should be aware that lay-offs scar not only workers but also their families, while the negative psychological effects could be long-lasting.