Skip to main content
Log in

Räumlicher Neglect

Spatial neglect

  • Weiterbildung · Zertifizierte Fortbildung
  • Published:
Der Nervenarzt Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Zusammenfassung

Unter dem Begriff (Hemi-)Neglect werden Symptome zusammengefasst, die mit (halbseitiger) Vernachlässigung des extra-, peri- und/oder personalen Raums einhergehen und nicht primär durch Störungen der Sensorik oder Motorik bedingt sind. Patienten mit Neglect haben Schwierigkeiten, ihre Aufmerksamkeit in die vernachlässigte kontraläsionale Raumhälfte zu wenden und auf visuelle, akustische oder somatosensorische Reize adäquat zu reagieren. Hemi-Neglect wird am häufigsten nach rechtshemisphärischen Läsionen, insbesondere des rechten inferioren Parietalkortex und des rechten temporoparietalen Übergangskortex beobachtet. Da eine adäquate Wahrnehmung und räumliche Repräsentation unseres Körpers und des uns umgebenden Raums notwendige Voraussetzungen für nahezu alle Aktivitäten des täglichen Lebens sind, führt Neglect zu einer Einschränkung der aktiven Teilnahme am Alltag und den Rehabilitationsprogrammen und ist deswegen mit einer schlechten funktionellen Prognose und einer stärkeren sozialen Beeinträchtigung assoziiert. Neue neuropsychologische und pharmakologische Therapieansätze versprechen, dass Neglect in Zukunft zielgerichteter behandelt werden kann.

Summary

Unilateral spatial (hemi-)neglect or (hemi-)inattention are clinical terms used to describe a number of different clinical symptoms which have in common the patient’s failure to attend to, respond adequately to, or orient voluntarily to people or objects in the contralesional space. Unilateral spatial neglect is most often observed following brain lesions affecting the right hemisphere, and in particular the right inferior parietal cortex (angular and supramarginal gyrus) and right temporoparietal junction. Importantly, the term hemineglect cannot be meaningfully used if the target behavior is explained by primary sensory or motor deficits only. Typically the patient’s deficit is supramodal: patients with hemineglect fail to respond to novel or meaningful stimuli irrespective of whether they are presented in the visual, auditory, and somesthetic (somatosensory) domain. As adequate perception and spatial representation of both the body and the outside world are mandatory for almost all activities of daily living, hemineglect is known to limit the degree of active participation in rehabilitation programs and is thus commonly associated with poor functional recovery and less successful social reintegration. A number of new promising behavioral and pharmacological treatments may help to ameliorate neglect in the future.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Abb. 1a–d
Abb. 2
Abb. 3a,b
Abb. 4
Abb. 5a–d
Abb. 6

Literatur

  1. Babinski J (1914) Contribution a l’etude de trouble mentaux dan l’hemiplegie organique cerebrale (Anosognosie). Rev Neurol (Paris) 27:845–848

    Google Scholar 

  2. Bisiach E, Luzzatti C (1978) Unilateral neglect of representational space. Cortex 14:129–133

    Google Scholar 

  3. Cappa SF, Sterzi R, Vallar G et al. (1987) Remission of hemineglect and anosognosia after vestibular stimulation. Neuropsychologia 25:775–782

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Corbetta M, Shulman GL (2002) Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:201–215

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Doricchi F, Guariglia C, Paolucci S et al. (1991) Disappearance of leftward rapid eye movements during sleep in left visual hemi-inattention. Neuroreport 2:285–288

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Driver J, Mattingley JB (1998) Parietal neglect and visual awareness. Nat Neurosci 1:17–22

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Fierro B, Brighina F, Oliveri M et al. (2000) Contralateral neglect induced by right posterior parietal rTMS in healthy subjects. Neuroreport 11:1519–1521

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Fink GR, Marshall JC, Shah NJ et al. (2000) Line bisection judgements implicate right parietal cortex and cerebellum as assessed by fMRI. Neurology 54:1324–1331

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Fink GR, Marshall JC, Weiss PH et al. (2002) Compensation for distorted egocentric representation of space implicates right inferior parietal cortex. Cortex 38:854–859

    Google Scholar 

  10. Fink GR, Marshall JC, Weiss PH et al. (2003) Performing allocentric visuospatial judgments with induced distortion of the egocentric reference frame: an fMRI study with clinical implications. Neuroimage 20:1505–1517

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Formisano E, Linden DE, Di Salle F et al. (2002) Tracking the mind’s image in the brain I: time-resolved fMRI during visuospatial mental imagery. Neuron 35:185–194

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Grossi D, Modafferi A, Pelosi L et al. (1989) On the different roles of the cerebral hemispheres in mental imagery: the „o’clock test“ in two clinical cases. Brain Cogn 10:18–27

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Halligan PW, Cockburn J (1993) Cognitive sequelae of stroke: visuospatial and memory disorders. Crit Rev Phys Rehabil Med 5:57–81

    Google Scholar 

  14. Halligan PW, Marshall JC (1991) Left neglect for near but not far space in man. Nature 350:498–500

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Halligan PW, Fink GR, Marshall JC et al. (2003) Spatial cognition: evidence from visual neglect. Trends Cogn Sci 7:125–133

    Google Scholar 

  16. Heide W, Kömpf D (1998) Combined deficits of saccades and visuo-spatial orientation after cortical lesions. Exp Brain Res 123:164–171

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Heilman KM, Watson RT (1991) Intentional motor disorders. In: Levin HS, Eisenberg HM, Benton AL (eds) Frontal lobe function and dysfunctions. Oxford University Press, New York

  18. Heilman KM, Bowers D, Coslett HB et al. (1985) Directional hypokinesia: prolonged reaction times for leftward movements in patients with right hemisphere lesions and neglect. Neurology 35:855–859

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Heilman KM, Valenstein E, Watson RT (2000) Neglect and related disorders. Semin Neurol 20:463–470

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Hilgetag CC, Theoret H, Pascual-Leone A (2001) Enhanced visual spatial attention ipsilateral to rTMS-induced ‚virtual lesions‘ of human parietal cortex. Nat Neurosci 4:953–957

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Husain M, Rorden C (2003) Non-spatially lateralized mechanisms in hemispatial neglect. Nat Rev Neurosci 4:26–36

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Husain M, Mannan SK, Hodgson T et al. (2001) Impaired spatial working memory across saccades contributes to abnormal search in parietal neglect. Brain 124:941–952

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Karnath H-O, Fetter M, Dichgans J (1996) Ocular exploration of space as a function of neck proprioceptive and vestibular input—observations in normal subjects and patients with spatial neglect after parietal lesions. Exp Brain Res 109:333–342

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Karnath HO, Ferber S, Himmelbach M (2001) Spatial awareness is a function of the temporal not the posterior parietal lobe. Nature 411:950–953

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Kerkhoff G (2003) Modulation and rehabilitation of spatial neglect by sensory stimulation. Prog Brain Res 142:257–271

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Kerkhoff G, Heidmann B (1999) Balint syndrome and associated disorders: anamnesis, diagnosis, approaches to treatment. Nervenarzt 70:859–869

    Google Scholar 

  27. Kinsbourne M (2003) Mechanisms of unilateral neglect. In: Jeannerod M (ed) Neurophysiological and neuropsychological aspect of spatial neglect. Elsevier Science Publishers, Amsterdam, pp 69–85

  28. Laplane D, Degos JD (1983) Motor neglect. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatr 46:152–158

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Marshall JC, Fink GR (2003) Cerebral localization, then and now. Neuroimage 20:S2–S7

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Marshall JC, Halligan PW (1995) Within- and between-task dissociations in visuo-spatial neglect: a case study. Cortex 31: 367–376

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Marshall JC, Fink GR, Halligan PW et al. (2002) Spatial awareness: a function of the posterior parietal lobe? Cortex 38:253–257

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Mesulam MM (1981) A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect. Ann Neurol 10:309–325

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Mort DJ, Malhotra P, Mannan SK et al. (2003) The anatomy of visual neglect. Brain 126:1986–1997

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Posner MI (1980) Orienting of attention. Q J Exp Psychol A 32:3–25

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Posner MI, Walker JA, Friedrich FJ et al. (1984) Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention. J Neurosci 4:1863–1874

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Robertson IH, Halligan PW (1999) Spatial neglect: a clinical handbook for diagnosis and treatment. Psychology Press, Hove

    Google Scholar 

  37. Robertson IH, North N (1992) Spatio-motor cueing in unilateral neglect: the role of hemispace, hand and motor activation. Neuropsychologia 30:553–563

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Robertson IH, Tegnér R, Tham K et al. (1995) Sustained attention training for unilateral neglect: theoretical and rehabilitation implications. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 17:416–430

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Robertson IH, Manly T, Beschin N et al. (1997) Auditory sustained attention is a marker of unilateral spatial neglect. Neuropsychologia 35:1527–1532

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Robertson IH, Ridgeway V, Greenfield E et al. (1997) Motor recovery after stroke depends on intact sustained attention: a 2-year follow-up study. Neuropsychology 11:290–295

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Rossetti Y, Rode G, Pisella L et al. (1998) Prism adaptation to a rightward optical deviation rehabilitates left visuospatial neglect. Nature 395:166–169

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Schindler I, Kerkhoff G, Karnath H-O et al. (2002) Neck muscle vibration induces lasting recovery in spatial neglect. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 73:412–419

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Sprenger A, Kömpf D, Heide W (2002) Visual search in patients with left visual hemineglect. Prog Brain Res 140:397–418

    Google Scholar 

  44. Stone SP, Patel P, Greenwood RJ et al. (1992) Measuring visual neglect in acute stroke and predicting its recovery. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 55:431–436

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Stone SP, Halligan PW, Greenwood RJ (1993) The incidence of neglect phenomena and related disorders in patients with an acute right or left hemisphere stroke. Age Ageing 22:46–52

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Vallar G (2001) Extrapersonal visual unilateral spatial neglect and its neuroanatomy. Neuroimage 14:S52–S58

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Vallar G, Perani D, Cappa SF et al. (1988) Recovery from aphasia and neglect after subcortical stroke: neuropsychological and cerebral perfusion study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 51:1269–1276

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Vallar G, Rusconi ML, Bignamini L et al. (1994) Anatomical correlates of visual and tactile extinction in humans: a clinical CT scan study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 57:464–470

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Vallar G, Guariglia C, Magnotti L et al. (1995) Optokinetic stimulation affects both vertical and horizontal deficits of position sense in unilateral neglect. Cortex 31:669–683

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Vallar G, Rusconi ML, Barozzi S et al. (1995) Improvement of left visuospatial hemineglect by left-sided transcutaneous electrical stimulation. Neuropsychologia 33:73–82

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Vogeley K, Fink GR (2003) Neural correlates of the first-person perspective. Trends Cogn Sci 7:38–42

    Google Scholar 

  52. Volpe BT, LeDoux JE, Gazzaniga MS (1979) Information processing of visual stimuli in an „extinguished“ visual field. Nature 22:724

    Google Scholar 

  53. Von Giesen HJ, Schlaug G, Steinmetz H et al. (1994) Cerebral network underlying unilateral motor neglect: evidence from positron emission tomography. J Neurol Sci 125:29–38

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Vuilleumier P, Valenza N, Mayer E et al. (1998) Near and far visual space in unilateral neglect. Ann Neurol 43:406–410

    Google Scholar 

  55. Walker R (1995) Spatial and object-based neglect. Neurocase 1:371–383

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Weiss PH, Marshall JC, Wunderlich G et al. (2000) Neural consequences of acting in near versus far space: a physiological basis for clinical dissociations. Brain 123:2531–2541

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Weiss PH, Marshall JC, Zilles K et al. (2003) Are action and perception in near and far space additive or interactive factors? Neuroimage 18:837–846

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Zimmermann P, Fimm B (1992) Testbatterie zur Aufmerksamkeitsprüfung (TAP). Psytest, Freiburg

  59. Weintraub S, Mesulam MM (1987) Right cerebral dominance in spatial attention. Further evidence based on ipsilateral neglect. Arch Neurol 44:621–625

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Rode G, Perenin MT, Boisson D (1995) [Neglect of the representational space: demonstration by mental evocation of the map of France]. Rev Neurol (Paris) 151:161–164

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Interessenkonflikt:

Der korrespondierende Autor versichert, dass keine Verbindungen mit einer Firma, deren Produkt in dem Artikel genannt ist, oder einer Firma, die ein Konkurrenzprodukt vertreibt, bestehen.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to G. R. Fink.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Fink, G.R., Heide, W. Räumlicher Neglect . Nervenarzt 75, 389–410 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00115-004-1698-3

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00115-004-1698-3

Schlüsselwörter

Keywords

Navigation