Skip to main content
Log in

The mimetic repertoire of the spotted bowerbird Ptilonorhynchus maculatus

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Naturwissenschaften Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although vocal mimicry in songbirds is well documented, little is known about the function of such mimicry. One possibility is that the mimic produces the vocalisations of predatory or aggressive species to deter potential predators or competitors. Alternatively, these sounds may be learned in error as a result of their acoustic properties such as structural simplicity. We determined the mimetic repertoires of a population of male spotted bowerbirds Ptilonorhynchus maculatus, a species that mimics predatory and aggressive species. Although male mimetic repertoires contained an overabundance of vocalisations produced by species that were generally aggressive, there was also a marked prevalence of mimicry of sounds that are associated with alarm such as predator calls, alarm calls and mobbing calls, irrespective of whether the species being mimicked was aggressive or not. We propose that it may be the alarming context in which these sounds are first heard that may lead both to their acquisition and to their later reproduction. We suggest that enhanced learning capability during acute stress may explain vocal mimicry in many species that mimic sounds associated with alarm.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bates HW (1862) Contributions to an insect fauna of the Amazon Valley (Lepidoptera, Heliconidae). Trans Linn Soc Lond 23:495–566

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baylis JR (1982) Avian vocal mimicry: its function and evolution. Acoustic communication in birds. Academic Press, Inc, New York, pp 51–80

    Google Scholar 

  • Beecher MD, Campbell SE et al (2000) Song-type matching between neighbouring song sparrows. Anim Behav 59:21–27

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buchanan KL, Spencer KA et al (2003) Song as an honest signal of past developmental stress in the European starling (Sturnus vulgaris). Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 270:1149–1156

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Charif RA, Clark CW et al (2004) Raven 1.2 user’s manual. Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology, Ithaca

    Google Scholar 

  • Chisholm AH (1932) Vocal mimicry among Australian birds. Ibis 13:605–625

    Google Scholar 

  • Chu M (2001) Heterospecific responses to scream calls and vocal mimicry by phainopeplas (Phainopepla nitens) in distress. Behaviour 138:775–787

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coe RL (2005) Vocal mimicry in the spotted bowerbird Chlamydera maculata. University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh

    Google Scholar 

  • Coleman SW, Patricelli GL et al (2007) Female preferences drive the evolution of mimetic accuracy in male sexual displays. Biol Lett 3(5):463–466

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conover MR (1994) Stimuli eliciting distress calls in adult passerines and response of predators and birds to their broadcast. Behaviour 131:19–37

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Kloet ER, Oitzl MS et al (1999) Stress and cognition: are corticosteroids good or bad guys? Trends Neurosci 22:422–426

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dobkin DS (1979) Functional and evolutionary relationships of vocal copying phenomena in birds. Z Fur Tierpsychol J Comp Ethol 50(4):348–363

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson JWH, Van Zyl A et al (2002) Vocal mimicry in African Cossypha robin chats. J Fur Ornithologie 143(3):319–330

    Google Scholar 

  • Flower T (2010) Fork-tailed drongos use deceptive mimicked alarm calls to steal food. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.1932

    Google Scholar 

  • Franco P, Slabbekoorn H (2009) Repertoire size and composition in great tits: a flexibility test using playbacks. Anim Behav 77:261–269

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frith CB, Frith DW (2004) The bowerbirds. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Garamszegi LZ, Eens M et al (2007) A comparative study of the function of heterospecific vocal mimicry in European passerines. Behav Ecol 18:1001–1009

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodale E, Kotagama SW (2006) Context-dependent vocal mimicry in a passerine bird. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 273(1588):875–880

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gorissen L, Gorissen M et al (2006) Heterospecific song matching in two closely related songbirds (Parus major and P. caeruleus): great tits match blue tits but not vice versa. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 60:260–269

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenlaw JS, Shackelford CE et al (1998) Call mimicry by eastern towhees and its significance in relation to auditory learning. Wilson Bull 110(3):431–434

    Google Scholar 

  • Harcus JL (1977) Functions of mimicry in vocal behavior of chorister robin. Z Fur Tierpsychol J Comp Ethol 44(2):178–193

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hausberger M, Jenkins PF et al (1991) Species-specificity and mimicry in bird song—are they paradoxes—a re-evaluation of song mimicry in the European starling. Behaviour 117:53–81

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hindmarsh AM (1986) The functional significance of vocal mimicry in song. Behaviour 99:87–100

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joëls M, Pu Z et al (2006) Learning under stress: how does it work? Trends Cogn Sci 10(4):152–158

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelley LA, Healy SD (2010) Vocal mimicry in male bowerbirds: who learns from whom? Biol Lett 6(5):626–629

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelley LA, Coe RL et al (2008) Vocal mimicry in songbirds. Anim Behav 76:521–528

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morton ES (1976) Observations of vocal mimicry in the thick-billed euphonia. Wilson Bull 88(3):485–487

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowicki S, Hasselquist D et al (2000) Nestling growth and song repertoire size in great reed warblers: evidence for song learning as an indicator mechanism in mate choice. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 267:2419–2424

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nowicki S, Searcy WA et al (2002) Brain development, song learning and mate choice in birds: a review and experimental test of the "nutritional stress hypothesis". J Comp Physiol A 188:1003–1014

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ratnayake CP, Goodale E et al (2010) Two sympatric species of passerine birds imitate the same raptor calls in alarm contexts. Naturwissenschaften 97:103–108

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rechten C (1978) Interspecific mimicry in birdsong—does Beau Geste hypothesis apply? Anim Behav 26:305–306

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson FN (1974) The function of vocal mimicry in some avian displays. Emu 74:9–10

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rowe MP, Coss RG et al (1986) Rattlesnake rattles and burrowing owl hisses: a case of acoustic Batesian mimicry. Ethology 72:53–71

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shors TJ, Weiss C et al (1992) Stress-induced facilitation of classical conditioning. Science 257:537–539

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Simpson K and Day N (1999) Field guide to the birds of Australia. Penguin Group

  • Snow BK (1974) Vocal mimicry in the violaceous euphonia, Euphonia violacea. Wilson Bull 86(2):179–180

    Google Scholar 

  • Spencer KA, Buchanan KL et al (2003) Song as an honest indicator or developmental history in the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata). Horm Behav 44:132–139

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stefanski RA, Falls JB (1972) A study of distress calls of song, swamp and white-throated sparrows (Aves: Fringillidae) I. Intraspecific responses and functions. Can J Zool 50(12):1501–1502

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thaker M, Vanak AT et al (2010) Stress and aversive learning in a wild vertebrate: the role of corticosterone in mediating escape from a novel stressor. Am Nat 175(1):50–60

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thorpe WH (1961) Bird song. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Vernon CJ (1973) Vocal imitation by southern African birds. Ostrich 44:23–30

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warham J (1962) Field notes on Australian bowerbirds and catbirds. Emu 62:1–30

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zann R, Dunstan E (2008) Mimetic song in superb lyrebirds: species mimicked and mimetic accuracy in different populations and age classes. Anim Behav 76:1043–1054

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank James Nicholls for his assistance both in and out of the field. We are grateful to QPWS for field site access and our field assistants for all their help. We thank Anne Goldizen for assistance with ethics and Tom Little for statistical advice. We thank two anonymous reviewers for their useful comments on this manuscript. LAK would like to thank NERC for funding. This research was approved by the University of Queensland Animal Ethics Committee (SIB/272/07/NERC & SIB/326/08/NERC) and the Queensland Environmental Protection Agency (WISP04545307 and WITK04545407).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Laura A. Kelley.

Additional information

Communicated by: Sven Thatje

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

ESM 1

(PDF 93 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kelley, L.A., Healy, S.D. The mimetic repertoire of the spotted bowerbird Ptilonorhynchus maculatus . Naturwissenschaften 98, 501–507 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00114-011-0794-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00114-011-0794-z

Keywords

Navigation