Abstract
Although vocal mimicry in songbirds is well documented, little is known about the function of such mimicry. One possibility is that the mimic produces the vocalisations of predatory or aggressive species to deter potential predators or competitors. Alternatively, these sounds may be learned in error as a result of their acoustic properties such as structural simplicity. We determined the mimetic repertoires of a population of male spotted bowerbirds Ptilonorhynchus maculatus, a species that mimics predatory and aggressive species. Although male mimetic repertoires contained an overabundance of vocalisations produced by species that were generally aggressive, there was also a marked prevalence of mimicry of sounds that are associated with alarm such as predator calls, alarm calls and mobbing calls, irrespective of whether the species being mimicked was aggressive or not. We propose that it may be the alarming context in which these sounds are first heard that may lead both to their acquisition and to their later reproduction. We suggest that enhanced learning capability during acute stress may explain vocal mimicry in many species that mimic sounds associated with alarm.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Bates HW (1862) Contributions to an insect fauna of the Amazon Valley (Lepidoptera, Heliconidae). Trans Linn Soc Lond 23:495–566
Baylis JR (1982) Avian vocal mimicry: its function and evolution. Acoustic communication in birds. Academic Press, Inc, New York, pp 51–80
Beecher MD, Campbell SE et al (2000) Song-type matching between neighbouring song sparrows. Anim Behav 59:21–27
Buchanan KL, Spencer KA et al (2003) Song as an honest signal of past developmental stress in the European starling (Sturnus vulgaris). Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 270:1149–1156
Charif RA, Clark CW et al (2004) Raven 1.2 user’s manual. Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology, Ithaca
Chisholm AH (1932) Vocal mimicry among Australian birds. Ibis 13:605–625
Chu M (2001) Heterospecific responses to scream calls and vocal mimicry by phainopeplas (Phainopepla nitens) in distress. Behaviour 138:775–787
Coe RL (2005) Vocal mimicry in the spotted bowerbird Chlamydera maculata. University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh
Coleman SW, Patricelli GL et al (2007) Female preferences drive the evolution of mimetic accuracy in male sexual displays. Biol Lett 3(5):463–466
Conover MR (1994) Stimuli eliciting distress calls in adult passerines and response of predators and birds to their broadcast. Behaviour 131:19–37
De Kloet ER, Oitzl MS et al (1999) Stress and cognition: are corticosteroids good or bad guys? Trends Neurosci 22:422–426
Dobkin DS (1979) Functional and evolutionary relationships of vocal copying phenomena in birds. Z Fur Tierpsychol J Comp Ethol 50(4):348–363
Ferguson JWH, Van Zyl A et al (2002) Vocal mimicry in African Cossypha robin chats. J Fur Ornithologie 143(3):319–330
Flower T (2010) Fork-tailed drongos use deceptive mimicked alarm calls to steal food. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.1932
Franco P, Slabbekoorn H (2009) Repertoire size and composition in great tits: a flexibility test using playbacks. Anim Behav 77:261–269
Frith CB, Frith DW (2004) The bowerbirds. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Garamszegi LZ, Eens M et al (2007) A comparative study of the function of heterospecific vocal mimicry in European passerines. Behav Ecol 18:1001–1009
Goodale E, Kotagama SW (2006) Context-dependent vocal mimicry in a passerine bird. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 273(1588):875–880
Gorissen L, Gorissen M et al (2006) Heterospecific song matching in two closely related songbirds (Parus major and P. caeruleus): great tits match blue tits but not vice versa. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 60:260–269
Greenlaw JS, Shackelford CE et al (1998) Call mimicry by eastern towhees and its significance in relation to auditory learning. Wilson Bull 110(3):431–434
Harcus JL (1977) Functions of mimicry in vocal behavior of chorister robin. Z Fur Tierpsychol J Comp Ethol 44(2):178–193
Hausberger M, Jenkins PF et al (1991) Species-specificity and mimicry in bird song—are they paradoxes—a re-evaluation of song mimicry in the European starling. Behaviour 117:53–81
Hindmarsh AM (1986) The functional significance of vocal mimicry in song. Behaviour 99:87–100
Joëls M, Pu Z et al (2006) Learning under stress: how does it work? Trends Cogn Sci 10(4):152–158
Kelley LA, Healy SD (2010) Vocal mimicry in male bowerbirds: who learns from whom? Biol Lett 6(5):626–629
Kelley LA, Coe RL et al (2008) Vocal mimicry in songbirds. Anim Behav 76:521–528
Morton ES (1976) Observations of vocal mimicry in the thick-billed euphonia. Wilson Bull 88(3):485–487
Nowicki S, Hasselquist D et al (2000) Nestling growth and song repertoire size in great reed warblers: evidence for song learning as an indicator mechanism in mate choice. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 267:2419–2424
Nowicki S, Searcy WA et al (2002) Brain development, song learning and mate choice in birds: a review and experimental test of the "nutritional stress hypothesis". J Comp Physiol A 188:1003–1014
Ratnayake CP, Goodale E et al (2010) Two sympatric species of passerine birds imitate the same raptor calls in alarm contexts. Naturwissenschaften 97:103–108
Rechten C (1978) Interspecific mimicry in birdsong—does Beau Geste hypothesis apply? Anim Behav 26:305–306
Robinson FN (1974) The function of vocal mimicry in some avian displays. Emu 74:9–10
Rowe MP, Coss RG et al (1986) Rattlesnake rattles and burrowing owl hisses: a case of acoustic Batesian mimicry. Ethology 72:53–71
Shors TJ, Weiss C et al (1992) Stress-induced facilitation of classical conditioning. Science 257:537–539
Simpson K and Day N (1999) Field guide to the birds of Australia. Penguin Group
Snow BK (1974) Vocal mimicry in the violaceous euphonia, Euphonia violacea. Wilson Bull 86(2):179–180
Spencer KA, Buchanan KL et al (2003) Song as an honest indicator or developmental history in the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata). Horm Behav 44:132–139
Stefanski RA, Falls JB (1972) A study of distress calls of song, swamp and white-throated sparrows (Aves: Fringillidae) I. Intraspecific responses and functions. Can J Zool 50(12):1501–1502
Thaker M, Vanak AT et al (2010) Stress and aversive learning in a wild vertebrate: the role of corticosterone in mediating escape from a novel stressor. Am Nat 175(1):50–60
Thorpe WH (1961) Bird song. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge
Vernon CJ (1973) Vocal imitation by southern African birds. Ostrich 44:23–30
Warham J (1962) Field notes on Australian bowerbirds and catbirds. Emu 62:1–30
Zann R, Dunstan E (2008) Mimetic song in superb lyrebirds: species mimicked and mimetic accuracy in different populations and age classes. Anim Behav 76:1043–1054
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank James Nicholls for his assistance both in and out of the field. We are grateful to QPWS for field site access and our field assistants for all their help. We thank Anne Goldizen for assistance with ethics and Tom Little for statistical advice. We thank two anonymous reviewers for their useful comments on this manuscript. LAK would like to thank NERC for funding. This research was approved by the University of Queensland Animal Ethics Committee (SIB/272/07/NERC & SIB/326/08/NERC) and the Queensland Environmental Protection Agency (WISP04545307 and WITK04545407).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
Communicated by: Sven Thatje
Electronic supplementary material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
ESM 1
(PDF 93 kb)
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Kelley, L.A., Healy, S.D. The mimetic repertoire of the spotted bowerbird Ptilonorhynchus maculatus . Naturwissenschaften 98, 501–507 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00114-011-0794-z
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00114-011-0794-z