Skip to main content
Log in

The Detrimental Effects of Extrinsic Reinforcement on “Intrinsic Motivation”

  • Published:
The Behavior Analyst Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Extrinsic consequences have been criticized on the grounds that they decrease intrinsic motivation or internally initiated behavior. Two popular rationales for this criticism, Lepper’s overjustification hypothesis (1981) and Deci’s motivational theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985), are reviewed and the criticism is then redefined behaviorally. “Intrinsically controlled” behavior is defined as behavior maintained by response-produced reinforcers, and the question concerning extrinsic consequences is thus restated as follows: When behavior is maintained by response-produced stimuli, does extrinsic reinforcement decrease the reinforcing value of those stimuli? The empirical support for this detrimental effect is summarized briefly, and several possible explanations for the phenomenon are offered. Research results that reflect on the effect’s generality and social significance are discussed next, with the conclusion that the effect is transient and not likely to occur at all if extrinsic rewards are reinforcing, noncompetitive, based on reasonable performance standards, and delivered repetitively.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Amabile, T. M. (1979). Effects of external evaluations on artistic creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 221–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amabile, T. M. (1982). Social psychology of creativity: A consensual assessment technique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 997–1013.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amabile, T. M. (1985). Motivation and creativity: Effects of motivational orientation on creative writers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 393–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amabile, T. M., De Jong, W., & Lepper, M. R. (1976). Effects of externally imposed deadlines on subsequent intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 92–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, R., Manoogian, S. T., & Reznick, J. S. (1976). The undermining and enhancing of intrinsic motivation in preschool children. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 915–922.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Antonitis, J. J., & Barnes, G. W. (1961). Group operant behavior: An extension of individual methodology to a real life situation. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 98, 95–111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arkes, H. R. (1979). Competence and the overjustification effect. Motivation and Emotion, 3, 143–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1987). Social foundations of thought and action. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D. E. (1955). The arousal and satiation of perceptual curiosity in the rat. Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology, 48, 238–246.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boggiano, A. K., & Ruble, D. N. (1979). Competence and the overjustification effect: A developmental study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1462–1468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Calder, B. J., & Staw, B. M. (1975). Self-perception of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 599–605.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Condry, J. (1977). Enemies of exploration: Self-initiated versus other-initiated learning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 459–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Danner, F. W., & Lonky, E. (1981). A cognitive-developmental approach to the effects of rewards on intrinsic motivation. Child Development, 52, 1043–1052.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, P., & Bucher, B. (1978). Intrinsic interest and extrinsic reward: The effects of a continuing token program on continuing noncon-strained preference. Behavior Therapy, 9, 222–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deal, R. M., & Madsen, C. H., Jr. (1980). Token systems and intrinsic motivation: An experimental analysis. Unpublished manuscript, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL.

    Google Scholar 

  • deCharms, R. (1968). Personal causation. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L. (1971). Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18, 105–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L. (1972a). Intrinsic motivation, extrinsic reinforcement, and inequity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 22, 113–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L. (1972b). The effects of contingent and noncontingent rewards and controls on intrinsic motivation. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 8, 217–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L. (1975a). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L. (1975b). Notes on the theory and metatheory of intrinsic motivation. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 15, 130–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L. (1978). Applications of research on the effects of rewards. In M. R. Lepper & D. Greene (Eds.), The hidden costs of reward: New perspectives on the psychology of motivation (pp. 193–203). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., Betley, G., Kahle, J., Abrams, L., & Porac, J. (1981). When trying to win: Competition and intrinsic motivation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 7, 79–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, A. M. (1985). The effects of reward and reinforcment on intrinsic interest. Dissertation Abstracts International 46, 3202B. (University Microfilms No. DA8524457.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunham, P. J. (1977). The nature of reinforcing stimuli. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp. 98–124). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Enzle, M. E., & Ross, J. M. (1978). Increasing and decreasing intrinsic interest with contingent rewards: A test of cognitive evaluation theory. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14, 588–597.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farr, J. L. (1976). Task characteristics, reward contingency, and intrinsic motivation. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 16, 294–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farr, J. L., Vance, R. J., & McIntyre, R. M. (1977). Further examinations of the relationship between reward contingency and intrinsic motivation. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 20, 31–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feingold, B. D., & Mahoney, M. J. (1975). Reinforcement effects on intrinsic interest: Undermining the overjustification hypothesis. Behavior Therapy, 6, 367–377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Franken, R. E. (1988). Human motivation. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks-Cole.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greene, D., & Lepper, M. R. (1974). Effects of extrinsic rewards on children’s subsequent intrinsic interest. Child Development, 45, 1141–1145.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, D., Sternberg, B., & Lepper, M. R. (1976). Overjustification in a token economy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 1219–1234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harackiewicz, J. M. (1979). The effects of reward contingency and performance feedback on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1352–1363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, H. F. (1950). Learning and satiation of response in intrinsically motivated complex puzzle performance by monkeys. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1950, 289–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, H. F., Harlow, M. K., & Meyer, D. R. (1950). Learning motivated by a manipulative drive. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40, 228–234.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hom, H. L., Jr., & Maxwell, F. R. (1980, April). Individual patterns in intrinsic motivation. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Southwestern Psychological Association, Oklahoma City, OK.

  • Horcones. (1983). Natural reinforcement in a Waiden Two community. Revista Mexicana de Analisis de la Conducta, 9, 141–143.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horcones. (1987). The concept of consequences in the analysis of behavior. The Behavior Analyst, 10, 291–294.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Karniol, R., & Ross, M. (1977). The effects of performance-relevant and performance-irrelevant rewards on children’s intrinsic motivation. Child Development, 48, 482–487.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kish, G. B. (1966). Studies of sensory reinforcement. In W. K. Honig (Ed.), Operant behavior: Areas of research and application (pp. 109–159). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kish, G. B., & Baron, A. (1962). Satiation of sensory reinforcement. Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology, 55, 1007–1010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kohn, A. (1988, January). Incentives can be bad for business. Inc., pp. 93–94.

  • Kruglanski, A. W., Alan, S., & Lewis, T. (1972). Retrospective misattribution and task enjoyment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 8, 493–501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., Riter, A., Amitai, A., Margolin, B., Shabtai, L., & Zaksh, D. (1975). Can money enhance intrinsic motivation? A test of the content-consequence hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 744–750.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lepper, M. R. (1981). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in children: Detrimental effects of superfluous social controls. In W. E. Collins (Ed.), Aspects of the development of competence (pp. 155–214). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lepper, M. R., & Greene, D. (1975). Turning play into work: Effects of adult surveillance and extrinsic rewards on children’s intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 479–486.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lepper, M. R., & Greene, D. (1976). On understanding “overjustification”: A reply to Reiss and Sushinsky. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, 25–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lepper, M. R., & Greene, D. (Eds.). (1978). The hidden costs of reward: New perspectives on the psychology of human motivation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lepper, M. R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1973). Undermining children’s intrinsic interest with extrinsic reward: A test of the “overjustification” hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28, 129–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levine, F. M., & Fasnacht, G. (1974). Token rewards may lead to token learning. American Psychologist, 29, 816–820.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loveland, K. K., & Olley, J. G. (1979). The effect of external reward on interest and quality of task performance in children of high and low intrinsic motivation. Child Development, 50, 1207–1210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luvten, H., & Lens, W. (1981). The effect of earlier experience and reward contingencies on intrinsic motivation. Motivation and Emotion, 5, 25–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, J. A. (1977). Effects of positive and negative adult-child interactions on children’s task performance and task preferences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 23, 493–502.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mawhinney, T. C. (1979). Intrinsic X extrinsic work motivation: Perspectives from behaviorism. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 24, 411–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mawhinney, T. C., Dickinson, A. M., & Taylor, L. A., III. (in press). The use of concurrent schedules to evaluate the effects of extrinsic rewards on “intrinsic motivation.” Journal of Organizational Behavior Management.

  • McClelland, D. C., Atkinson, J. W., Clark, R. W., & Lowell, E. L. (1953). The achievement motive. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • McGraw, K. O., & Fiala, J. (1982). Undermining the Zeigarnik effect: Another hidden cost of reward. Journal of Personality, 50, 58–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGraw, K., & McCullers, J. (1979). Evidence of a detrimental effect of extrinsic incentives on breaking a mental set. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 15, 285–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Montgomery, K. C. (1952). Exploratory behavior and its relation to spontaneous alternation in a series of maze exposures. Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology, 45, 50–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, M. (1983). Decrements in intrinsic interest among rewarded and observer subjects. Child Development, 54, 636–644.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, M. (1984). Reward-induced decrements and increments in intrinsic motivation. Review of Educational Research, 54, 5–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinder, C. C. (1976). Additivity versus nonadditivity of intrinsic and extrinsic incentives: Implications for work motivation, performance, and attitudes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 61, 693–700.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pittman, T. S., Davey, M. E., Alafat, K. A., Wetherill, K. V., & Kramer, N. A. (1980). Informational versus controlling verbal rewards. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 6, 228–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pittman, T. S., & Heller, J. F. (1987). Social motivation. Annual Review of Psychology, 38, 461–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Porac, J. F., & Meindl, J. (1982). Undermining overjustification: Inducing intrinsic and extrinsic task representations. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 29, 208–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pritchard, R. D., Campbell, K. M., & Campbell, D. J. (1977). Effects of extrinsic financial rewards on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 62, 9–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reiss, S., & Sushinsky, L. W. (1975). Overjustification, competing responses, and the acquisition of intrinsic interest. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 1116–1125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenfield, D., Folger, R., & Adelman, H. F. (1980). When rewards reflect competence: A qualification of the overjustification effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 368–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, M. (1975). Salience of reward and intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 245–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, M., Karniol, R., & Rothstein, M. (1976). Reward contingency and intrinsic motivation in children: A test of the delay of gratification hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, 442–447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M. (1982). Control and information in the intrapersonal sphere: An extension of cognitive evaluation theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 450–461.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., Mims, B., & Koestner, R. (1983). Relation of reward contingency and interpersonal context to intrinsic motivation: A review and test using cognitive evaluation theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 736–750.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salancik, G. R. (1975). Interaction effects of performance and money on self-perception of intrinsic motivation. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 13, 339–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, W. E., Jr. (1975). The effects of extrinsic rewards on “intrinsic motivation.” Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 25, 311–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, W. E., Jr. (1980). The hidden costs of reward remain largely hidden. Contemporary Psychology, 25, 106–107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shapira, Z. (1976). Expectancy determinants of intrinsically motivated behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 1235–1244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1969). Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical analysis. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1974). About behaviorism. New York: Vintage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, M. C. (1975). Children’s use of the multiple sufficient cause schema in social perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 737–747.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swann, W. B., Jr., & Pittman, T. S. (1977). Initiating play activity of children: The moderating influence of verbal cues on intrinsic motivation. Child Development, 48, 1128–1132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vasta, R., Andrews, D. E., McLaughlin, A. M., Stirpe, L. A., & Comfort, C. (1978). Reinforcement effects on intrinsic interest: A classroom analog. Journal of School Psychology, 16, 161–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vasta, R., & Stirpe, L. A. (1979). Reinforcement effects on three measures of children’s interest in math. Behavior Modification, 3, 223–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vaughan, M. E., & Michael, J. (1982). Automatic reinforcement: An important but ignored concept. Behaviorism, 10, 217–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, M. J. (1980). The effect of incentive and control over outcomes upon intrinsic motivation and performance. The Journal of Social Psychology, 112, 247–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, M. J., & Mander, A. M. (1978). The effects of reward and perception of competency upon intrinsic motivation. Motivation and Emotion, 2, 67–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welker, W. I. (1956). Some determinants of play and exploration in chimpanzees. Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology, 49, 84–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66, 297–333.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, B. W. (1980). Reinforcement, behavior constraint, and the overjustification effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 599–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J. (1985). The development of “intrinsic” motivation: A social learning analysis. Annals of Child Development, 2, 117–160.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Dickinson, A.M. The Detrimental Effects of Extrinsic Reinforcement on “Intrinsic Motivation”. BEHAV ANALYST 12, 1–15 (1989). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392473

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392473

Key words

Navigation