Skip to main content
Log in

Positive and negative reinforcement: Should the distinction be preserved?

  • Published:
The Behavior Analyst Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Michael (1975) reviewed efforts to classify reinforcing events in terms of whether stimuli are added (positive reinforcement) or removed (negative reinforcement). He concluded that distinctions in these terms are confusing and ambiguous. Of necessity, adding a stimulus requires its previous absence and removing a stimulus its previous presence. Moreover, there is no good basis, either behavioral or physiological, that indicates the involvement of distinctly different processes, and on these grounds he proposed that the distinction be abandoned. Despite the cogency of Michael’s analysis, the distinction between positive and negative reinforcement is still being taught. In this paper, we reconsider the issue from the perspective of 30 years. However, we could not find new evidence in contemporary research and theory that allows reliable classification of an event as a positive rather than a negative reinforcer. We conclude by reiterating Michael’s admonitions about the conceptual confusion created by such a distinction.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baron, A. (1991). Avoidance and punishment. In I. H. Iversen & K. A. Lattal (Eds.), Techniques in the behavioral and neural sciences: Vol. 6. Experimental analysis of behavior (Part 1, pp. 173–217). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, A., DeWaard, R. J., & Galizio, M. (1981). Factor-analytically derived subscales for the reinforcer survey schedule: Reinforcer preference as a function of drug use and sex. Behavior Modification, 5, 203–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, A., & Kaufman, A. (1966). Human freeoperant avoidance of “time-out” from monetary reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9, 557–565.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, J. E., & Katz, J. L. (1981). Drug effects on behaviors maintained by different events. In T. Thompson, P. B. Dews, & W. A. McKim (Eds.), Advances in behavioral pharmacology (Vol. 3, pp. 119–165). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, J. M., Knutson, B., Fong, G. W. Caggiano, D. M. Bennett, S. M., & Hummer, D. W. (2004). Incentive-elicited brain activation in adolescents: Similarities and differences from young adults. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 1793–1802.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. F., & Hendy, S. (2001). A step toward ending the isolation of behavior analysis: A common language with evolutionary science. The Behavior Analyst, 24, 163–171.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Catania, A. C. (1973). The nature of learning. In J. A. Nevin & G. S. Reynolds (Eds.), The study of behavior (pp. 31–68). Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catania, A. C. (1998). Learning (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cautela, J. R. (1972). Reinforcement survey schedule: Evaluation and current applications. Psychological Reports, 30, 683–690.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crowley, T. J. (1972). The reinforcers for drug abuse: Why people take drugs. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 13, 51–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • D’Amato, M. R. (1969). Instrumental conditioning. In M. H. Marx (Ed.), Learning processes (pp. 35–118). New York: MacMillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, M., & Astrachan, M. I. (1978). Conditioned fear and startle magnitude: Effects of different footshock or backshock intensities used in training. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 4, 95–103.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, M., Campeau, S., Kim, M., & Falls, W. A. (1995). Neural systems of emotion: The amygdala’s role in fear and anxiety. In J. L. McGaugh, N. M. Weinberger, & G. Lynch (Eds.), Brain and memory: Modulation and mediation of neuroplasticity (pp. 3–40). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, M. E., Schell, A. M. & Boehmelt, A. H. (Eds.). (1999). Startle modification: Implications for neuroscience, cognitive science and clinical science. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dworkin, S. I., Pitts, R., & Galizio, M. (1993). Schedule-controlled behavior: Negative reinforcement. In F. van Haaren (Ed.), Methods in behavioral pharmacology (pp. 101–116). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Farber, P. D., Khavari, K. A., & Douglass, F. M. (1980). A factor analytic study of reasons for drinking: Empirical validation of positive and negative reinforcement dimensions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48, 780–781.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Friman, P. C., Hayes, S. C., & Wilson, K. G. (1998). Why behavior analysts should study emotion: The example of anxiety. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 137–156.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Galbicka, G., & Platt, J. R. (1984). Interresponse-time punishment: A basis for shock-maintained behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 41, 291–308.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hilgard, E. R., & Marquis, D. G. (1940). Conditioning and learning. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hineline, P. N. (1984). Aversive control: a separate domain? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 42, 495–509.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Iversen, I. H., & Lattal, K. A. (Eds.). (1991). Techniques in the behavioral and neural sciences: Vol. 6. Experimental analysis of behavior. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iwata, B. A,, Pace, G. M., Dorsey, M. F., Zarcone, J. R., Vollmer, T. R., Smith, R. G., et al. (1994). The functions of self-injurious behavior: An experimental-epidemiological analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 215–240.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kelleher, R. T., & Morse, W. H. (1964). Escape behavior and punished behavior. Federation Proceedings, 23, 808–817.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keller, F. S., & Schoenfeld, W. N. (1950). Principles of psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiyatkin, E. A. (1995). Functional significance of mesolimbic dopamine. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 19, 573–598.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knutson, B., Burgdorf, J., & Panksepp, J. (2002). Ultrasonic vocalizations as indices of affective states in rats. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 961–977.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lamal, P. A. (1998). Advancing backwards. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 705–706.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Laurence, M. T., Hineline, P. N., & Bersh, P. J. (1994). The puzzle of responding maintained by response-contingent shock. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 61, 135–153.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lundqvist, L. (1995). Facial expressions are contagious. Journal of Psychophysiology, 9, 203–211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mazur, J. E. (2002). Learning and behavior (5th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCullough, L. D., Sokolowski, J. D., & Salamone, J. D. (1993). A neurochemical and behavioral investigation of the involvement of nucleus accumbens dopamine in instrumental avoidance. Neuroscience, 52, 919–925.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Michael, J. (1975). Positive and negative reinforcement: A distinction that is no longer necessary; or a better way to talk about bad things. Behaviorism, 3, 33–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morse, W. H., & Kelleher, R. T. (1977). Determinants of reinforcement and punishment, In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp. 174–200). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mowrer, O. H. (1960). Learning theory and behavior. New York: Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp, J., Sacks, D. S., Crepeau, L. J., & Abbot, B. B. (1991). The psycho- and neurobiology fear systems in the brain. In M. R. Denny (Ed.), Fear, avoidance and phobias: A fundamental analysis (pp. 7–60). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pear, J. J. (2001). The science of learning. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perone, M. (2003). Negative effects of positive reinforcement. The Behavior Analyst, 26, 1–14.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Perone, M., & Galizio, M. (1987). Variable-interval schedules of timeout from avoidance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 47, 97–113.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Pierce, W. D., & Cheney, C. D. (2004). Behavior analysis and learning (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ritz, T., Dahme, B., & Claussen, C. (1999). Gradients of facial EMG and cardiac activity during emotional simulation. Journal of Psychophysiology, 13, 3–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salamone, J. D., Correa, M., Mingote, S., & Weber, S. M. (2003). Nucleus accumbens dopamine and the regulation of effort in food-seeking behavior: Implications for studies of natural motivation, psychiatry and drug abuse. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 305, 1–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salamone, J. D., Cousins, M. S., & Snyder, B. J. (1997). Behavioral functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine: Empirical and conceptual problems with the anhedonia hypothesis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 21, 341–359.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1989). Coercion and its fallout. Boston: Authors Cooperative.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: MacMillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1976). About behaviorism. New York: Vintage Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1981). Selection by consequences. Science, 213, 501–504.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1986). What is wrong with daily life in the Western world? American Psychologist, 41, 568–574.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Small, D. M., Zatorre, R. J., Dagher, A., Evans, A. C., & Jones-Gotman, M. (2001). Changes in brain activity related to eating chocolate. Brain, 124, 1720–1733.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, R. L. (1980). The opponent process theory of acquired motivation. American Psychologist, 35, 691–712.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, R. L., & Corbit, J. D. (1974). An opponent process theory of motivation: I. The temporal dynamics of affect. Psychological Review, 81, 119–145.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, G. C. (1961). Nondiscriminated avoidance behavior in human subjects. Science, 133, 641–642.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L. (1911). Animal intelligence: Experimental studies. New York: Macmillan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Vaccarino, F. J., Schiff, B. B., & Glickman, S. E. (1989). Biological view of reinforcement. In S. B. Klein & R. R. Mowrer (Eds.), Contemporary learning theories: Instrumental conditioning theory and the impact of biological constraints on learning theory (pp. 111–142). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verhave, T. (1962). The functional properties of a time out from an avoidance schedule. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 5, 391–422.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, B., & Laties, V. G. (1961). Behavioral thermoregulation. Science, 133, 1338–1344.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wise, R. A., & Bozarth, M. A. (1987). A psychomotor stimulant theory of addiction. Psychological Review, 94, 469–492.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Alan Baron or Mark Galizio.

Additional information

We thank Marshall Dermer, Rob Hakan, Jay Moore, Michael Perone, and Ray Pitts for helpful comments on earlier versions of this manuscript. Preparation of this article was supported by Grant DA 012879 from the National Institutes of Health.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Baron, A., Galizio, M. Positive and negative reinforcement: Should the distinction be preserved?. BEHAV ANALYST 28, 85–98 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392107

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392107

Key words

Navigation