Advertisement

The Psychological Record

, Volume 40, Issue 4, pp 517–539 | Cite as

Some Effects of Response Cost on a Free-Operant Multiple-Response Repertoire with Humans

  • John Crosbie
Article
  • 1 Downloads

Abstract

Two experiments with human subjects were performed to investigate the effects of response cost on several unpunished free-operant responses. Using monetary gain on variable-interval schedules as reinforcement, a multiple-response repertoire of button-press responses was developed, then response cost was made contingent on one of these responses. A multielement baseline design was employed in both experiments: During the baseline phase both multielement components were correlated with baseline conditions, then one component was correlated with response cost while baseline conditions continued in the other component. In Experiment 1, a five-response repertoire was developed with no changeover delay (COD), and response cost decreased the target response, increased most nontarget responses, and, for two subjects, one nontarget response decreased. In Experiment 2, a four-response repertoire was developed with a 5-s COD, and similar results were obtained except that no nontarget responses decreased. The present results support my previous findings that punishment affects the whole set of unpunished responses (Crosbie, 1988, 1989, 1990), No support was found, however, for Dunham’s hierarchical rule and most frequent follower propositions (Dunham, 1978; Dunham & Grantmyre, 1982).

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. AZRIN, N. H., & HOLZ, W. C. (1966). Punishment. In W. K. Honig (Ed.), Operant behavior: Areas of research and application (pp. 380–447). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  2. BARLOW, D. H., HAYES, S. C, & NELSON, R. O. (1984). The scientist practitioner: Research and accountability in clinical and educational settings. New York: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
  3. BRADSHAW, C. M., SZABADI, E., & BEVAN, P. (1977). Effect of punishment on human variable-interval performance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 27, 275–279.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  4. BRADSHAW, C. M., SZABADI, E., & BEVAN, P. (1978). Effect of variable-interval punishment on the behavior of humans in variable-interval schedules of monetary reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 29, 161–166.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  5. CATANIA, A. C, & REYNOLDS, G. S. (1968). A quantitative analysis of the responding maintained by interval schedules of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11, 327–383.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  6. CHURCH, R. M. (1969). Response suppression. In B. A. Campbell & R. M. Church (Eds.), Punishment and aversive behavior (pp. 111–156). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  7. CROSBIE, J. (1988). The behavioural side-effects of human punishment. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Flinders University of South Australia, Adelaide.Google Scholar
  8. CROSBIE, J. (1989). The effects of punishment on unpunished reinforced free-operant responses. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  9. CROSBIE, J. (1990). The effects of punishment on unpunished behaviour. Behaviour Change, 7, 25–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. CROSBIE, J., & SHARPLEY, C. F. (1989a). Dmitsa: A simplified interrupted time-series analysis program. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 21, 639–642.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. CROSBIE, J., & SHARPLEY, C. R (1989b). The Dmitsa manual. Unpublished manuscript, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia.Google Scholar
  12. DELUTY, M. Z. (1976). Choice and the rate of punishment in concurrent schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 25, 75–80.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  13. DE VILLIERS, P. A. (1977). Choice in concurrent schedules and a quantitative formulation of the law of effect. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp. 233–287). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  14. DE VILLIERS, P. A. (1981). Quantitative studies of punishment: The negative law of effect revisited. In C. M. Bradshaw, E. Szabadi, & C. F. Lowe (Eds.), Quantification of steady-state operant behaviour (pp. 139–151). Amsterdam: Elsevier/North-Holland Biomedical Press.Google Scholar
  15. DINSMOOR, J. A. (1954). Punishment: I. The avoidance hypothesis. Psychological Review, 61, 34–46.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. DUNHAM, P. J. (1971). Punishment: Method and theory. Psychological Review, 78, 58–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. DUNHAM, P. J. (1972). Some effects of punishment upon unpunished responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 17, 443–450.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  18. DUNHAM, P. J. (1978). Changes in unpunished responding during response-contingent punishment. Animal Learning and Behavior, 6, 174–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. DUNHAM, P. J., CORNWALL, A., & HURSHMAN, A. (1986). Structural features in multiple response repertoires: Effects of response restriction. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 40, 12–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. DUNHAM, P. J., & GRANTMYRE, J. (1982). Changes in a multiple-response repertoire during response-contingent punishment and response restriction: Sequential relationships. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 123–133.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  21. ELSMORE, T. F. (1971). Effects of response effort on discrimination performance. The Psychological Record, 21, 17–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. ESTES, W. K. (1944). An experimental study of punishment. Psychological Monographs, 57(3), Whole No. 263.Google Scholar
  23. GELLERMANN, L. W. (1933). Chance orders of alternating stimuli in visual discrimination experiments. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 42, 206–208.Google Scholar
  24. GOTTMAN, J. M. (1981). Time-series analysis: A comprehensive introduction for social scientists. London: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  25. HARRIS, S. L, & ERSNER-HERSHFIELD, R. (1978). Behavioral suppression of seriously disruptive behavior in psychotic and retarded patients: A review of punishment and its alternatives. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 1352–1375.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. HONIG, W. K., & SLIVKA, R. M. (1964). Stimulus generalization of the effects of punishment. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 7, 21–25.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  27. NEWSOM, C, FAVELL, J. E., & RINCOVER, A. (1983). The side effects of punishment. In S. Axelrod & J. Apsche (Eds.), The effects of punishment on human behavior (pp. 285–316). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. PREMACK, D. (1965). Reinforcement theory. In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation, 1965 (pp. 123–188). University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
  29. SIDMAN, M. (1960). Tactics of scientific research: Evaluating experimental data in psychology. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  30. SIEGEL, S. (1956). Nonparametric statistics for the behaviorai sciences. Tokyo: McGraw-Hill Kogakusha.Google Scholar
  31. SKINNER, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: An experimental analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  32. SKINNER, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  33. SOLOMON, R. L. (1964). Punishment. American Psychologist, 19, 239–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. THORNDIKE, E. L. (1911). Animal intelligence: An experimental study of the associative processes in animals. Psychological Monographs, 2(4). Whole No. 8.Google Scholar
  35. THORNDIKE, E. L. (1932). Fundamentals of learning. New York: Teachers College.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. THORNDIKE, E. L. (1933). A proof of the law of effect. Science, 77, 173–175.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. TODOROV, J. C, HANNA, E. S., & BITTENCOURT DE SÁ, M. C. N. (1984). Frequency versus magnitude of reinforcement: New data with a different procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 41, 157–167.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  38. ULMAN, J. D., & SULZER-AZAROFF, B. (1975). Multielement baseline design in educational research. In E. Ramp & G. Semp (Eds.), Behavior analysis: Areas of research and application (pp. 377–391). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  39. WEINER, H. (1962). Some effects of response cost on human operant behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 5, 201–208.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  40. WEINER, H. (1964a). Response cost and fixed-ratio performance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 7, 79–81.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  41. WEINER, H. (1964b). Response cost effects during extinction following fixed-interval reinforcement in humans. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 7, 333–335.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Association of Behavior Analysis International 1990

Authors and Affiliations

  • John Crosbie
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyDeakin UniversityAustralia

Personalised recommendations