Advertisement

The Psychological Record

, Volume 32, Issue 1, pp 19–27 | Cite as

Sustained Blood Pressure Responding During Synthetic Work

  • R. L. Ray
  • H. H. Emurian
Article

Abstract

Hour-long elevations in mean blood pressure were elicited by the performance of a synthetic work task for both naive subjects and experienced subjects. Task-elicited changes in heart rate failed to reach significance although split-half reliabilities of both heart rate measures and mean blood pressure measures were very high during task performance. Several significant correlations were observed between performance and cardiovascular response magnitude that differed in sign for the experienced and naive groups. The use of the synthetic work task as a tool for the study of sustained blood pressure elevations elicited by work stress is discussed.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. ALLUISI, E.A., & CHILES, W.D. 1967. Sustained performance, work-rent scheduling, and diurnal rhythms in man. Acta Psychological, 27, 436–442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. ANDERSON, D.E., & BRADY, J.V. 1971. Preavoidance blood pressure elevations accompanied by heart rate decreases in the dog. Science, 172, 595–597.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. De LEON, G. 1972. Classical conditioning and extinction of human systolic blood pressure. Conditional Reflex, 7, 193–209.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. ELLIOT, R. 1974. The motivational significance of heart rate. In P.A. Obrist, A.H. Black, J. Brener, & L.V. DiCara (Eds.), Cardiovascular psychophysiology. Chicago: Aldine.Google Scholar
  5. EMURIAN, H.H. 1978. A multiple-task performance battery presented on a cathode ray tube. Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 8, 102–103.Google Scholar
  6. EMURIAN, H.H., & BRADY, J.V. 1979. Small group performance and the effects of contingency management in a programmed environment: A progress report. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 9.Google Scholar
  7. GEDDES, L.A., MOORE, A.G., GARNER, H., ROSBOROUGH, J., ROSS, J., & AMEND, J. 1970. The indirect measurement of blood pressure in the horse. The Southwestern Veterinarian, 23, 289–294.Google Scholar
  8. GENTRY, W.D. 1970. Sex differences in the effects of frustration and attack on emotion and vascular processes. Psychological Reports, 27, 383–390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. LACEY, J.I., & LACEY, B.C. 1970. Some autonomie-central nervous system interrelationships. In P. Black (Ed.), Physiological correlates of emotion. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  10. MORGAN, B.B., & ALLUISI, E.A. 1972. Synthetic work: Methodology for assessment of human performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 35, 835–845.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. OBRIST, P.A. 1963. Cardiovascular differentiation of sensory stimuli. Psychosomatic Medicine, 25, 450–459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. OBRIST, P.A. 1976. The cardiovascular-behavioral interaction—as it appears today. Psychophysiology, 13, 95–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. SCHACHTER, J. 1957. Pain, fear, and anger in hypertensives and normotensives. Psychosomatic Medicine, 19, 17–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. VAN EGEREN, L.F. 1979. Social interactions, communications, and the coronary-prone behavior pattern: A psychophysiological study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 41, 2–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Association of Behavior Analysis International 1982

Authors and Affiliations

  • R. L. Ray
    • 1
  • H. H. Emurian
    • 1
  1. 1.Phipps Clinic 261The Johns Hopkins University School of MedicineBaltimoreUSA

Personalised recommendations