Skip to main content
Log in

The Role of Intraverbal Exchanges in Assessing Parent-Child Relationships

  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present investigation evaluated the role of verbal exchanges between parent and child (intraverbal exchanges) in relation to two contemporary measurements of the parent-child relationship, mutual responsive orientation (MRO) and synchrony. Data were collected from 30 mother-preschool child dyads (19 girls, 11 boys) during a laboratory assessment. Rates of intraverbal exchange, MRO, and a major aspect of synchrony, dyadic reciprocity, were obtained during four separate interactions: two structured puzzle tasks, a semi-structured play activity, and an unstructured snack period. Results indicated that the rate of intraverbal exchange was stable within dyads and highly correlated with the two other, more complex measures. The findings challenge the restricted view of the utility of the behavior analytic approach, implicitly maintained in socialization and attachment theories within developmental psychology, demonstrate the value of measures of intraverbal exchanges for assessing parent-child relationships, and suggest practical applications in parent and caregiver training.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • ABIDIN, R. R. (1995). Parenting Stress Index Software Portfolio (Ps -Sp). Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

    Google Scholar 

  • AINSWORTH, M. D. S., BELL, S. M., & STAYTON, D. (1974). Infant-mother attachment and social development. In M. P. Richards (Ed.), The introduction of the child into a social world (pp. 99–135). London: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • AKSAN, N., KOCHANSKA, G., & ORTMANN, M. R. (2006). Mutually responsive orientation between parents and their young children: Toward methodological advances In the science of relationships. Developmental Psychology, 42, 833–848. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.42.5.833

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • AUERBACH, J. G., FAROY, M., & EBSTEIN, R. (2001). The association of the dopamine D4 receptor gene (Drd4) and the serotonin transporter gene (5-Httlpr) with temperament In 12-month infants. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 777–783. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00774

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • BELSKY, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: A process model. Child Development, 55, 83–96. doi:10.2307/1129836

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • BUGENTHAL, D. B., & GRUSEC, J. E. (2006). Socialization theory. In N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (pp. 366–428). New York: Wiley

    Google Scholar 

  • CALKINS, S. D., FOX, N. A., & MARSHALL, T. R. (1996). Behavioral and physiological antecedents of inhibition In infancy. Child Development, 67, 523–540. doi:10.2307/1131830

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • CHASE, P. N., JOHNSON, K. R., & SULZER-AZAROFF, B. (1985). Verbal relations within instruction: Are there subclasses of the intraverbal? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43, 301–313. doi:10.1901/jeab.1985.43-301

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • CHOMSKY, N. (1959). Review of B. F. Skinner’s Verbal Behavior. Language, 35, 26–58. doi:10.2307/411334

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • CHOMSKY, N. (1972). Language and mind. New York: Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • COYL, D., ROGGMAN, L., & NEWLAND, L. (2002). Stress, maternal depression, and negative mother-infant interactions In relation to infant attachment. Infant Mental Health Journal, 23, 145–160. doi:10.1002/imhj.10009

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • CRYSTAL, D. (2006). How language works: How babies babble, words change meaning, and language lives or dies. Woodstock, NY: Overlook Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • DALE, R., & SPIVEY, M. J. (2006). Unraveling the dyad: Using recurrence analysis to explore patterns of syntactic coordination between children and caregivers In conversation. Language Learning, 56, 391–440. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9922.2006.00372.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DYMOND, S., O’HORA, D., WHELAN, R., & O’DONOVAN, A. (2006). Citation analysis of Skinner’s Verbal Behavior: 1984—2004. The Behavior Analyst, 29, 75–88.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • FOX, N. A., CALKINS, S. D., PORGES, S. W., RUBIN, K., COPLAN, R. J., STEWART, S., et Al. (1995). Frontal activation asymmetry and social competence at four years of age. Child Development, 66, 1770–1784. doi:10.2307/1131909

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • GARROD, S., & PICKERING, M. J. (2004). Why is conversation so easy? Trends In Cognitive Sciences, 8, 8–11. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.10.016

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • GERARD, A. B. (1994). Parent-Child Relationship Inventory. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • GOLDSTEIN, M. H., KING, A. P., & WEST, M. J. (2003). Social interaction shapes babbling: Testing parallels between birdsong and speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100, 8030–8035. doi:10.1073/pnas.1332441100

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • GREER, R. D. (2008). The ontogenetic selection of verbal capabilities: Contributions of Skinner’s verbal behavior theory to a more comprehensive understanding of language. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 8, 363–386.

    Google Scholar 

  • GREER, R. D., & LONGANO, J. (2010). Naming a rose: How we may learn to do it. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 26, 73–106.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • GREER, R. D., & ROSS, D. E. (2008). Verbal behavior analysis: Developing and expanding verbal capabilities In children with language delays. Boston: Allyn & Bacon/Merrill.

    Google Scholar 

  • GREER, R. D., & SPECKMAN, J. (2009). The integration of speaker and listener responses: A theory of verbal development. The Psychological Record, 59, 449–488.

    Google Scholar 

  • HARRIST, A. W., PETTIT, G. S., DODGE, K. A., & BATES, J. E. (1994). Dyadic synchrony In mother-child interaction: Relation with children’s subsequent kindergarten adjustment. Family Relations, 43, 417–424. doi:10.2307/585373

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • HARRIST, A. W., & WAUGH, R. M. (2002). Dyadic synchrony: Its structure and function In child development. Developmental Review, 22, 555–592. doi:10.1016/S0273-2297(02)00500-2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • HART, B., & RISLEY, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences In the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Paul H. Bookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • HAYES, S. C., BARNES-HOLMES, D., & ROCHE, B. (Eds.). (2001). Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • INGVARSSON, E. T., TIGER, J. H., HANLEY, G. P., & STEPHENSON, K. M. (2007). An evaluation of intraverbal training to generate socially appropriate responses to novel questions. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40, 411–429. doi:10.1901/jaba.2007.40-411

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • KOCHANSKA, G. (1997). Mutually responsive orientation between mothers and their young children: Implications for early socialization. Child Development, 68, 112. doi:10.2307/1131928

    Google Scholar 

  • KOCHANSKA, G. (2002). Mutually responsive orientation between mothers and their young children: A context for the early development of conscience. Current Directions In Psychological Science, 11, 191–195. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00198

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • KOCHANSKA, G., AKSAN, N., PRISCO, T. R., & ADAMS, E. (2008). Mother-child and father- child mutually responsive orientation In the first 2 years and children’s outcome at preschool: Mechanisms of influence. Child Development, 79, 30–44. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01109.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • KOCHANSKA, G., & MURRAY, K. T. (2000). Mother-child mutually responsive orientation and conscience development: From toddler to early school age. Child Development, 71, 417–431. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00154

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • KNAPP, T. J. (1992). Verbal Behavior: The other reviews. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 10, 87–95.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • KUCZYNSKI, L. (2003). Beyond bidirectionality: Bilateral conceptual frameworks for understanding dynamics In parent-child relations. In L. Kuczynski (Ed.), Handbook of dynamics In parent-child relations (pp. 3–24). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • LAIBLE, D. J., & THOMPSON, R. A. (2000). Mother-child discourse, attachment, security, shared positive affect, and early conscience. Child Development, 71, 1424–1440. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00237

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LEIGLAND, S. (2007). Fifty years later: Comments on the further development of a science of verbal behavior. The Behavior Analyst Today, 8, 336–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LINDSEY, E. W., CREMEENS, P. R., COLWELL, M. J., & CALDERA, Y. M. (2008). The structure of parent-child dyadic synchrony In toddler hood and children’s communication competence and self-control. Social Development, 18, 375–396. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00489.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LODHI, S., & GREER, R. D. (1989). The speaker as listener. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 353–360. doi:10.1901/jeab.1989.51-353

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • MACCOBY, E. E. (1999). The uniqueness of the parent-child relationship. In W. A. Collins & B. Laursen (Eds.), Minnesota symposium on child psychology: Vol. 30. Relationships as developmental contexts (pp. 157–175). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • MACCORQUODALE, K. (1969). B. F. Skinner’s Verbal Behavior: A retrospective appreciation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 831–841. doi:10.1901/jeab.1969.12-831

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • MACCORQUODALE, K. (1970). On Chomsky’s review of Skinner’s Verbal Behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 13, 83–99. doi:10.1901/jeab.1970.13-83

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • MIZE, J., & PETTIT, G. S. (1997). Mother’s social coaching, mother-child relationship style, and children’s peer competence: Is the medium the message? Child Development, 68, 312–332. doi:10.2307/1131852

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • ONTAI, L. L., & THOMPSON, R. A. (2002). Patterns of attachment and maternal discourse effects on children’s emotion understanding from 3- to 5-years of age. Social Development, 11, 433–450. doi:10.1111/1467-9507.00209

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ONTAI, L. L., & THOMPSON, R. A. (2008). Attachment, parent-child discourse and theory of mind development. Social Development, 17, 47–60. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00414.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • PALMER, D. C. (2006). On Chomsky’s appraisal of Skinner’s Verbal Behavior. The Behavior Analyst, 29, 253–267.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • PARTINGTON, J. W., & BAILEY, J. S. (1993). Teaching of intraverbal behavior to preschool children. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 11, 9–18.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • PAULUSSEN-HOOGEBOOM, M. C., STAMS, G. J. J. M., HERMANNS, J. M. A., & PEETSMA, T. T. D. Relations among child negative emotionality, parenting stress, and maternal sensitive responsiveness In early childhood. Parenting: Science and Practice, 8, 1–16. doi:10.1080/15295190701830656

  • PEREZ-GONZALEZ, L. A., HERSZLIKOWICZ, K., & WILLIAMS, G. (2008). Stimulus relations analysis and the emergence of novel intraverbal. The Psychological Record, 58, 95–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • PINKER, S. (1994). The language instinct. New York: Harper.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • PREMACK, D. (1976). Intelligence In ape and man. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • PREMACK, D. (2010). Why humans are unique: Three theories. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 22–32. doi:10.1177/1745691609356782

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • SAUTTER, R. A., & LEBLANC, L. A. (2006). Empirical applications of Skinner’s analysis of verbal behavior with humans. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 22, 35–48.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SCHLINGER, H. D. (2002). Not so fast, Mr. Pinker: A behaviorist looks at The Blank Slate. A review of Steven Pinker’s The Blank Slate: The Modern Denial of Human Nature. Behavior and Social Issues, 12, 75–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SCHLINGER, H. D. (2008). The long good-bye: Why B. F. Skinner’s Verbal Behavior is alive and well on the 50th anniversary of its publication. The Psychological Record, 58, 329–337.

    Google Scholar 

  • SCHOLMERICH, A., FRACASSO, M. P., LAMB, M. E., & BROBERG, A. G. (1995). Interactional harmony at 7 and 10 months of age predicts security of attachment as measured by Q-sort ratings. Social Development, 4, 62–74. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.1995.tb00051.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M. (1994). Equivalence relations and behavior: A research story. Boston: Authors Cooperative.

    Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M. (2000). Equivalence relations and the reinforcement contingency. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 74, 127–146. doi:10.1901/jeab.2000.74-127

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SKINNER, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. doi:10.1037/11256-000

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • SKINNER, B. F. (1987). Controversy? In S. Modgil & C. Modgil (Eds.), B. F. Skinner: Consensus and controversy (pp. 11–12). New York: Falmer.

    Google Scholar 

  • STEPHENS, G. J., SILBERT, L. J., & HASSON, U. (2010). Speaker-listener neural coupling underlies successful communication. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 14425–14430. doi:10.1073/pnas.1008662107

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SUNDBERG, M. L., & MICHAEL, J. (2001). The benefits of Skinner’s analysis of verbal behavior In children with autism. Behavior Modification, 25, 698–724. doi:10.1177/0145445501255003

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • THOMPSON, R. A. (2010). Feeling and understanding through the prism of relationships. In S. D. Calkins & M. A. Bell (Eds.), Child development at the intersection of emotion and cognition (pp. 79–95). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/12059-005

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • TRONICK, E. z., EDWARD, M., ALS, H., & BRAZELTON, T. B. (1977). Mutuality In mother-infant interaction. Journal of Communication, 27, 74–79. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.1977.tb01829.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • WELCH-ROSS, M. (1997). Mother-child participation In conversation about the past: Relationships to preschoolers’ theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 33, 618–629. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.33.4.618

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • WHORF, B. L. (1956). Science and linguistics. In J. B. Carroll (Ed.), Language, thought, and reality: Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. Cambridge, MA: Mit Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • WYATT, W. J., HAWKINS, R. P., & DAVIS, P. (1986). Behaviorism: Are reports of its death exaggerated? The Behavior Analyst, 9, 101–105

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nurper Gökhan.

Additional information

This research was funded by a CUNY Collaborative Incentive Grant (80209–06–12) to the authors.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gökhan, N., Dennis, T.A. & Crossman, A.M. The Role of Intraverbal Exchanges in Assessing Parent-Child Relationships. Psychol Rec 62, 207–224 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395798

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395798

Key words

Navigation