Skip to main content
Log in

Salt-Induced Thirst Results in Increased Finickiness in Humans

  • Article
  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Common sense suggests that water-deprived or food-deprived organisms should be more willing to consume foods or fluids that would be deemed undesirable under lower states of deprivation. With food, evidence favoring this account has been observed; however other studies find that hungry participants demonstrate increased finickiness—avoiding less palatable alternatives and consuming more of pleasant ones. This study set out to test whether thirst generally increases acceptability (the “common sense” view) or whether it produces increased finickiness. Thirst was induced in one group of participants (relative to nonthirsty controls), and the impact of this on their judgment of optimal and suboptimal water sources was examined. Thirsty participants liked optimal stimuli more, but liked suboptimal stimuli less, relative to controls, but this occurred only when the stimuli were actually tasted. These data suggest that thirst polarizes extant hedonic responses to fluids—finickiness—thereby maintaining optimal water selection under conditions of mild to moderate deprivation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • AUBERT, A., & DANZER, R. (2005). The taste of sickness: lipopolysaccharide-induced finickiness in rats. Physiology and Behavior, 84, 437–444.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • BAUER, L. M., & ALTARRIBA, J. (2008). An investigation of sex differences in word ratings across concrete, abstract and emotion words. The Psychological Record, 58, 465–474.

    Google Scholar 

  • CHANGIZI, M. A., & HALL, W. G. (2001). Thirst modulates a perception. Perception, 30, 1489–1497.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DESS, N. K., CHAPMAN, C. D., & MINOR, T. R. (1988). Inescapable shock increases finickiness about drinking quinine-adulterated water in rats. Learning and Motivation, 19, 408–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DUCLAUX, R., FEISTHAUER, J., & CABANAC, M. (1973). effets du repas sur l’agrement d’odeurs alimentaires et nonalimentaires chez l’homme. Physiology and behavior, 10, 1029–1033.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DYKENS, E. M. (2000). Contaminated and unusual food combinations: What do people with Prader-Willi syndrome choose? Mental Retardation, 38, 163–171.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • EASTERBROOK, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66, 183–201.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • GLENDINNING, J. I. (1994). Is the bitter rejection response always adaptive? Physiology and behavior, 56, 1217–1227.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • HAIDT, J., McCAULEY, C., & ROZIN, P. (1994). Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: A scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 701–713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • HOEFLING, A., LIKOWSKI, K. U., HAFNER, M., SEIBT, B., MUHLBERGER, A., WEYERS, P., & STRACK, F. (2009). When hunger finds no fault with moldy corn: Food deprivation reduces food-related disgust. Emotion, 9, 50–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • JACOBS, H. l., & SHARMA, K. N. (1969). Taste versus calories: Sensory and metabolic signals in the control of food intake. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 157, 1084–1125.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • KAPUSCINSKI, R. (2008). Shadow of the sun. Melbourne, Australia: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • KAUFFMAN, N. A., HERMAN, P., & POLIVY, J. (1995). Hunger-induced finickiness in humans. Appetite, 24, 203–218.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LANG, P. J., BRADLEY, M. M., & CUTHBERT, B. N. (2001). International affective picture system (Iaps): Instruction manual and affective ratings (Tech. Rep. A-5). Miami: The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.

    Google Scholar 

  • MINOR, T. R., DESS, N. K., BEN-DAVID, E., & CHANG, W. (1994). Individual differences in vulnerability to inescapable shock in rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 20, 402–412.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • PLINER, P., HERMAN, C. P., & POLIVY, J. (1990). Palatability as a determinant of eating: Finickiness as a function of taste, hunger and the prospect of good food. In E. D. CAPALDI & T. L. POWLEY (eds.)? Taste, experience and feeding (pp. 210–226). Washington, DC: Apa.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • ROLLS,. J., & ROLLS, E. T. (1982). Thirst. Cambridge, Uk: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • ROLLS,. J., WOODS, R. J., ROLLS, E. T., LIND, H., LIND, W., & LEDINGHAM, J. G. G. (1980). Thirst following water deprivation in humans. American Journal of Physiology, 23, R476–482.

    Google Scholar 

  • ROLLS,. T., & ROLLS, J. H. (1997). Olfactory sensory-specific satiety in humans. Physiology and behavior, 61, 461–473.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • SCHACHTER, S. (1971). Some extraordinary facts about obese humans and rats. American Psychologist, 26, 129–145.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • SORENSEN, L. B., MOLLER, P., FLINT, A., MARTENS, M., & RABEN, A. (2003). effect of sensory perception of foods on appetite and food intake: A review of studies on humans. International Journal of Obesity, 27, 1152–1166.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • STEVENSON, R. J., CASE, T. I. & TOMICZEK, C. (2007). Resistance to interference of olfactory perceptual learning. The Psychological Record, 57, 103–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • VALDIVIA, S., LUCIANO, C., & MOLINA, F. J. (2006). verbal regulation of motivational states. The Psychological Record, 56, 577–595.

    Google Scholar 

  • ZELLNER, D. A., STEWART, W. F., ROZIN, P., & BROWN, J. M. (1988). effect of temperature and expectations on liking for beverages. Physiology and Behavior, 44, 61–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Richard J. Stevenson.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Stevenson, R.J., Case, T.I. & Oaten, M.J. Salt-Induced Thirst Results in Increased Finickiness in Humans. Psychol Rec 60, 385–398 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395717

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395717

Key words

Navigation