Skip to main content
Log in

Consequence Valuing as Operation and Process: A Parsimonious Analysis of Motivation

  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The concept of the motivating operation (MO) has been subject to 3 criticisms: (a) the terms and concepts employed do not always overlap with traditional behavior-analytic verbal practices; (b) the dual nature of the MO is unclear; and (c) there is a lack of adequate contact with empirical data. We offer a more parsimonious approach to motivation, extending Skinner’s (1938, 1953) early work on this topic, and introduce two new key terms: consequence-valuing operation (CVO) and consequence-valuing process (CVP). Our account focuses on the value of the reinforcer in terms of the level of responding that occurs relative to some previously measured baseline. We suggest that the concepts of the CVO and CVP will facilitate the identification, analysis, and manipulation of motivational factors in the treatment of behavior disorders.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • ADELINIS, J. D., PIAZZA, C. C., FISHER, W. W., & HANLEY, G. P. (1997). The establishing effects of client location on self-injurious behavior. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 18, 383–391.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • ALLYON, T., & Azrin, N. H. (1968). Reinforcer sampling: A technique for increasing the behavior of mental patients. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 13–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • AUSTIN, M. P., MITCHELL, P., & GOODWIN, G. M. (2001). Cognitive defects in depression: Possible implications for neuropathology. British Journal of Psychiatry, 178, 200–206.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • CATANIA, C. A. (1992). Learning (3rd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, Nj: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • CATANIA, C. A. (1993). Coming to terms with establishing operations. The Behavior Analyst, 16, 219–224.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • CHERPAS, C. (1993). Do establishing operations alter reinforcement effectiveness? The Behavior Analyst, 16, 347–349.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • CHIESA, M. (1994). Radical behaviorism: The philosophy and the science. Boston, Ma: Authors Cooperative.

    Google Scholar 

  • CONNERS, J., IWATA, B. A., KAHNG, S., HANLEY, G. P., WORSDELL, A. S., & THOMPSON, R. H. (2000). Differential responding in the presence and absence of discriminative stimuli during multielement functional analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 299–308.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • DELEON, I. G., FISHER, W. W., CATTER, V. R., MAGLIERI, K., HERMAN, K., & MARHEFKA, J. (2001). Examination of relative reinforcement effects of stimuli identified through pretreatment and daily brief preference assessments. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, 463–473.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • DOUGHER, M. J. (1983). Clinical effects of response deprivation and response satiation procedures. Behavior Therapy, 14, 286–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GULLEY, V., & NORTHUP, J. (1997). Comprehensive school-based behavioral assessment of the effects of methylphenidate. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 627–638.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • HANLEY, G. P., IWATA, B. A., LINDBERG, J. S., & CONNERS, J. (2003). Response-restriction analysis: I. Assessment of activity preferences. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 47–58.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • HANLEY, G. P., IWATA, B. A., ROSCOE, E. M., THOMPSON, R. H., & LINDBERG, J. S. (2003). Response-restriction analysis: II. Alteration of activity preferences. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 59–76.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • HAYES, S. C., KOHLENBERG, B. S., & HAYES, L. J. (1991). The transfer of specific and general consequential functions through simple and conditional equivalence relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of behavior, 56, 119–137.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • HINELINE, P. N. (1980). The language of behavior analysis: Its community, its functions, and its limitations. Behaviorism, 8, 67–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • HORNER, R. H., DAY, M. H., & DAY, J. R. (1997). Using neutralizing routines to reduce problem behaviors. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 601–613.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • HURSH, S. R. (1978). The economics of daily consumption controlling food-and water-reinforced responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of behavior, 29, 475–491.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • IWATA, B. A., SMITH, R. G., & MICHAEL, J. L. (2000). Current research on the influence of establishing operations on behavior in applied settings. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 411–418.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • KELLER, F. S., & SCHOENFELD, W. N. (1950). Principles of psychology. New york: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • KLATT, K. P., & MORRIS, E. K. (2001). The Premack principle, response deprivation, and establishing operations. The Behavior Analyst, 24, 173–180.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • LARAWAY, S., SNYCERSKI, S., MICHAEL, J., & POLING, A. (2001/2002). The abative effect: A new term to describe the action of antecedents that reduce operant responding. The Analysis of Verbal behavior, 18, 101–104.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • LARAWAY, S., Snycerski, S., Michael, J., & Poling, A. (2003). Motivating operations and terms to describe them: Some further refinements. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 407–414.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • LEIGLAND, S. (1984). On “setting events” and related concepts. The Behavior Analyst, 7, 41–45.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • MCDEVITT, M. A., & Fantino, E. (1993). Establishing operations and the discriminative stimulus. The Behavior Analyst, 16, 225–227.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • MCGILL, P. (1999). Establishing operations: Implications for the assessment, treatment, and prevention of problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 393–418.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • MCPHERSON, A., & Osborne, J. G. (1988). Control of behavior by an establishing stimulus. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of behavior, 49, 213–227.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • MICHAEL, J. (1982). Distinguishing between discriminative and motivational functions of stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 149–155.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • MICHAEL, J. (1983). Evocative and repertoire-altering effects of an environmental event. The Analysis of Verbal behavior, 2, 19–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MICHAEL, J. (1988). Establishing operations and the mand. The Analysis of Verbal behavior, 2, 19–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MICHAEL, J. (1993a). Establishing operations. The Behavior Analyst, 16, 191–206.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • MICHAEL, J. (1993b). Concepts and principles of behavior analysis. Kalamazoo, Mi: Society for the Advancement of Behavior Analysis.

    Google Scholar 

  • MICHAEL, J. (2000). Implications and refinements of the establishing operation concept. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 401–410.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • MICHAEL, J., Hixson, M. D., & Clark, J. W. (1997). The role of motivation in the S-R issue. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of behavior, 67, 239–241.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • MURPHY, E. S., MCSWEENEY, F. K., SMITH, R. G., & MCCOMAS, J. J. (2003). Dynamic changes in reinforcer effectiveness: Theoretical, methodological, and practical implications for applied research. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 421–438.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • PERRY, A.C., & FISHER, W. W. (2001). Behavioral economic influences on treatments designed to decrease destructive behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, 211–215.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SKINNER, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: An experimental analysis. Oxford: Appleton-Century.

    Google Scholar 

  • SKINNER, B. F. (1945). The operational analysis of psychological terms. Psychological Review, 52, 270–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SKINNER, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New york: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • SKINNER, B. F. (1999). The genetic nature of the concepts of stimulus and response. I? Cumulative record: Definitive edition (pp. 504-524). Acton, Ma: Copley. (Original work published 1935).

    Google Scholar 

  • SMITH, R. G., & IWATA, B. A. (1997). Antecedent influences on behavior disorders. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 343–375.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • STONER, G., CAREY, S., IKEDA, M., & SHINN, M. (1994). The utility of curriculum-based measurement for evaluating the effects of methylphenidate on academic performance. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 101–113.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • VALDIVIA, S., LUCIANO, C., & MOLINA, F. J. (2006). Verbal regulation of motivational states. The Psychological Record, 56, 577–595.

    Google Scholar 

  • VOLLMER, T. R., & IWATA, B. A. (1991). Establishing operations and reinforcement effects. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 279–291.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • WHELAN, R., & BARNES-HOLMES, D. (2004). The transformation of consequential functions in accordance with the relational frames of Same and Opposite. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of behavior, 82, 177–195.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • WHELAN, R., BARNES-HOLMES, D., & DYMOND, S. (2006). The transformation of consequential functions in accordance with the relational frames of more-than and less-than. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of behavior, 86, 317–335.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Robert Whelan.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Whelan, R., Barnes-Holmes, D. Consequence Valuing as Operation and Process: A Parsimonious Analysis of Motivation. Psychol Rec 60, 337–354 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395711

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395711

Key words

Navigation