Skip to main content
Log in

Competing Contingencies Over Derived Relational Responding: A Behavioral Model of the Implicit Association Test

  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Subjects were exposed to a word-picture association training phase in which each of 2 arbitrary nonsense syllables printed in blue and red font, respectively, were paired with either sexual or aversive photographic images. Subjects were then exposed to an equivalence training procedure that led to the formation of 2 3-member equivalence relations, each containing 1 of the 2 nonsense syllables in their respective color fonts, and 2 novel nonsense syllables in black font. in effect, equivalence class 1 (blue) was associated with sexual images, while equivalence class 2 (red) was associated with aversive images. Subjects were then exposed to a 2-block test in which sexual and aversive images and all members of the trained equivalence relations, presented in black font, were employed. in 1 block, subjects were instructed to produce responses that were compatible with their laboratory history Specifically subjects were instructed to produce the same operant response on a computer keyboard upon the presentation of both sexual images and members of equivalence class 1 (blue), and to produce another common response upon the presentation of aversive images and members of equivalence class 2 (red). in the second block of the test the instructions were juxtaposed such that subjects were required to produce common responses to members of classes that were not previously associated with one another (e.g., sexual images and members of equivalence class 2, red). Differences in the fluency of performances across both blocks of the final test were sensitive to subjects’ relational and conditioning histories. That is, subjects produced significantly more correct responses during block 1 of the test compared to block 2. Such findings lay the foundation for the development of functionally understood behavioral tests and provide a functional-analytic model of the widely used Implicit Association Test.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • BANSE, R., SEISE, J., & ZERBES, N. (2001). Implicit attitudes toward homosexuality: Reliability, validity, and controllability of the Iat. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 145–160.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • BARNES, D. (1994). Stimulus equivalence and relational frame theory. The Psychological Record, 44, 91–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • BARNES, D., LAWLOR, H., SMEETS, P., & ROCHE, B. (1996). Stimulus equivalence and academic self-concept among mildly mentally handicapped and nonhandicapped children. The Psychological Record, 46, 87–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • BENTALL, R. P., DICKINS, D. W., & FOX, S. R. A. (1993). Naming and equivalence: Response latencies for emergent relations. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46B, 187–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • COHEN, J. (1994). The Earth is round (p <.05). American Psychologist, 49, 997–1003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • COHEN J. D., MACWHINNEY B., FLATT M., & PROVOST J. (1993). PsyScope: A new graphic interactive environment for designing psychology experiments. Behavioral Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 25, 257–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DE HOUWER, J., (2001). A structural and process analysis of the Implicit Association test. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 443–451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DE HOUWER, J. (2006). What are implicit measures and why are we using them? In R. W. Wiers & A. W. Stacy (Eds.), The handbook of implicit cognition and addiction (pp. 11–28). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publishers.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • DEVOS, T., & BANAJI, M. R. (2005). American = white? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 447–466.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DIXON, M. R., DYMOND, S., REHFELDT, R. A., ROCHE, B., & ZLOMKE, K. R. (2003). Terrorism and relational frame theory. Behavior and Social Issues, 12, 129–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • FIEDLER, K. A., MESSNER, C., & BLUEMKE, M. (2006). Unresolved problems with the “I”, the “A”, and the “T”: A logical and psychometric critique of the Implicit Association Test (Iat). European Review of Social Psychology, 17, 74–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • FIELDS, L., ADAMS, B. J., VERHAVE, T., & NEWMAN, S. (1990). The effects of nodality on the formation of equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 53, 345–358.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • GREENWALD, A. G., MCGHEE, D. E., & SCHWARTZ, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • GREENWALD, A. G., NOSEK, B. A., & BANAJI, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • GREY, I., & BARNES, D. (1996). Stimulus equivalence and attitudes. The Psychological Record, 46, 243–270.

    Google Scholar 

  • JOHNSTON, J. M., & PENNYPACKER, H. S. (1993). Readings for strategies and tactics of behavioral research (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • KARPINSKI, A., & HILTON, J. L. (2001). Attitudes and the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 774–788.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • KEENAN, M., MCGLINCHEY, A., FAIRHURST, C., & DILLENBERGER, K. (2000). Accuracy of disclosure and contextual control in child abuse: developing procedures within the stimulus equivalence paradigm. Behavior and Social Issues, 10, 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • KIM, D. (2003). Voluntary controllability of the Implicit Association test (Iat). Social Psychology Quarterly, 66, 83–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LANG, P. J., BRADLEY, M. M., & CUTHBERT, B. N. (1999). International Affective Picture System: Instruction manual and affective ratings. Technical Report A-4, The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.

    Google Scholar 

  • LESLIE, J. C., TIERNEY, K. J., ROBINSON, C. P., KEENAN, M., WATT, A., & BARNES, D. (1993). Differences between clinically anxious and non-anxious subjects in a stimulus equivalence training task involving threat words. The Psychological Record, 43, 153–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • MCGLINCHEY, A., KEENAN, M., & DILLENBURGER, K. (2000). Outline for the development of a screening procedure for children who have been sexually abused. Research on Social Work Practice, 10, 721–747.

    Google Scholar 

  • MERWIN, R. M., & WILSON, K. G. (2005). Preliminary findings on the effects of self-referring and evaluative stimuli on stimulus equivalence class formation. The Psychological Record, 55, 561–575.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MITCHELL, C. J., ANDERSON, N. E., & LOVIBOND, P. F. (2003). Measuring evaluative conditioning using the Implicit Association Test. Teaming and Motivation, 34, 203–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’HORA, D., ROCHE, B., BARNES-HOLMES, D., & SMEETS, P. M. (2002). Response latencies to multiple derived stimulus relations: Testing two predictions of relational frame theory. The Psychological Record, 52, 51–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ROCHE, B., LINEHAN, C., WARD, T., DYMOND, S., REHELDT, R. (2004). The unfolding of the relational operant: A real-time analysis using electroencephalography and reaction time measures. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 4, 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • ROCHE B., RUIZ, M. R., & HAND, K. (2003). An experimental analysis of social discrimination using relational frame theory. Paper presented at the Annual Conference of the Association for Behavior Analysis, San Francisco, May 23–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • ROCHE, B., RUIZ, M., O’RIORDAN, M., & HAND, K. (2005). A relational frame approach to the psychological assessment of sex offenders. In M. Taylor & E. Quayle (Eds.), Viewing child pornography on the Internet: Understanding the offence, managing the offender, and helping the victims (pp. 109–125). Dorset, UK: Russell House Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • ROTHERMUND, K., & WENTURA, D. (2004). Underlying processes in the Implicit Association Test (Iat): Dissociating salience from associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 139–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M. (1960). Tactics of scientific research. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M. (1986). Functional analysis of emergent verbal classes. In T. Thompson & M. E. Zeiler (Eds.), Analysis and integration of behavioural units (pp. 213–245). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • SKINNER, B. F. (1950). Are theories of learning necessary? Psychological Review, 57, 193–216.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • SPENCER, T. J., & CHASE, P. N. (1996). Speed analyses of stimulus equivalence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 643–659.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • STEELE, D., & HAYES, S. C. (1991). Stimulus equivalence and arbitrarily applicable relational responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 519–555.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • STEFFENS, M., & PLEWE, I. (2001). Items’ cross-category associations as a confounding factor in the Implicit Association Test. Zeitschrift Euer Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 123–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • STEWART, L., BARNES-HOLMES, D., ROCHE, B., & SMEETS, P. (2002). A functional-analytic model of analogy: A relational frame analysis. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 78, 375–396.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • WATT, A., KEENAN, M., BARNES, D., & CAIRNS, E. (1991). Social categorization and stimulus equivalence. The Psychological Record, 41, 33–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • WULFERT, E., GREENWAY, D., & DOUCHER, M. (1994). Third-order equivalence classes. The Psychological Record, 44, 411–439.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • WULFERT, E., & HAYES, S. C. (1988). The transfer of conditional ordering response through conditional equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 125–144.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Amanda Gavin.

Additional information

This article is based on papers presented at the International Association for Behavior Analysis Annual Convention, Boston, 2004, and the International Association for Behavior Analysis Annual Convention, Chicago, 2005.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gavin, A., Roche, B. & Ruiz, M.R. Competing Contingencies Over Derived Relational Responding: A Behavioral Model of the Implicit Association Test. Psychol Rec 58, 427–441 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395627

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395627

Navigation