Skip to main content
Log in

Generalized Break Equivalence II: Contextual Control Over A Generalized Pattern of Stimulus Relations

  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In an earlier work (Gomez, Barnes-Holmes, & Luciano, 2001), it was found that although generalized break equivalence was achieved for 3 subjects (i.e., responding to new stimulus sets producing symmetry and transitivity but not equivalence; Cx-Ay, Cy-Ax), only 1 subject showed this pattern of responding during the first test exposure, and 2 additional subjects did not produce the derived pattern. The present study attempted to develop a procedure that produces generalized break equivalence more effectively than in our previous research. To achieve this goal three experiments were conducted varying the contextual cues used for the abstraction of the Generalized Break Equivalence Pattern (Gbep). In Experiment 1, the main contextual cue consisted of one, two, three, or four asterisks to differentiate each type of relation (i.e., one for A-B, B-A; two for B-C, C-B; three for A-C; four for C-A). Four subjects produced the Gbep, and the most frequent errors were produced on the A-C relation. In Experiment 2 the contextual cue was that the C stimuli were formally different from the A and B stimuli. Three subjects produced the Gbep and the most common errors were produced on C-B relation. Finally, in Experiment 3 an “explicit” contextual cue appeared only on C-A trials. All subjects showed the Gbep (2 of them in the first test to which they were exposed).These data extend those found earlier (Gömez et al., 2001) and support the generalized operant nature of derived relational responding. Applied implications are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • BARNES, Q., & Roche, B. (1996). Relational frame theory and stimulus equivalence are fundamentally different: A reply to Saunders’ commentary. The Psycological Record, 46, 489–507.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • BARNES-HOLMES, D., & BARNES-HOLMES, Y. (2000). Explaining complex behavior: Two perspectives on the concept of generalized operant classes. The Psychological Record, 50, 251–265.

    Google Scholar 

  • BOELENS, H. (1996). Accounting for stimulus equivalence: Reply to Hayes and Wilson. The Psychological Record, 46, 237–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • BUFFINGTON, D. M., FIELDS, L., & ADAMS, B. J. (1997). Enhancing equivalence class formation by pretraining of other equivalence classes. The Psychological Record, 47, 69–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • BUSH, K. M., SIDMAN, M., & De ROSE, T. (1989). Contextual control of emergent equivalence relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 29–45.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • DUBE, W. V., & MCILVANE, W. J. (1996). Some implications of a stimulus control topography analysis for emergent behavior and stimulus classes. In T. R. ZENTALL & R M. SMEETS (Eds.), Stimulus class formation in humans and animals (pp. 197–217). Elsevier Science B.V.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • DYMOND, S., & BARNES, D. (1995). A transformation of self-discrimination response functions in accordance with the arbitrarily applicable relations of sameness, more-than, and less-than. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 64, 163–184. Erratum, 66, 348.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • ETZEL, B. C., & LEBLANC, J. M. (1979). The simplest treatment alternative: The law of parsimony applied to choosing appropriate instructional control and errorless-learning procedures for the difficult-to-teach child. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 9, 361–382.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • FIELDS, L., VARELAS, A., REEVE, K. F., BELANICH, J., WADHWA, P., DEROSE, P., & ROSEN, D. (2000). Effects of prior conditional discrimination training, symmetry, transitivity, and equivalence testing on the emergence of new equivalence classes. The Psychological Record, 50, 443–466.

    Google Scholar 

  • FOLLETTE, W. C. (1998). Expanding the domains of clinical behaviorism: A comment on Dougher. Behavior Therapy, 29, 593–601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GAROTTI, M., DESOUZA, D. G., DEROSE, J. C, MOLINA, R. C., & GIL, M. S. A. (2000). Reorganization of equivalence classes after reversal of baseline relations. The Psychological Record, 50, 35–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • GATCH, M. B., & OSBORNE, J. G. (1989). Transfer of contextual stimulus function via equivalence class development. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 369–378.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • GOMEZ, S., BARNES-HOLMES, D., & LUCIANO, M. C. (2001). Generalized break equivalence I. The Psychological Record, 51, 131–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • GOMEZ, S., HUERTA, E., BARNES, D., LUCIANO, M. C, & SMEETS, P. M. (1999). Breaking equivalence relations. Experimental Analysis of Human Behavior Bulletin, 173, 1–4.

    Google Scholar 

  • GREEN, G., STROMER, R., & MACKAY, H. A. (1993). Relational learning in stimulus sequences. The Psychological Record, 43, 599–615.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • HAYES, S. C. (1994). Relational frame theory: A functional approach to verbal events. In S. C. HAYES, L. J. HAYES, M. SATO, & K. Ono (Eds.), Behavior Analysis of Language and Cognition (pp. 9–30). Reno, Nv: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • HAYES, S. C., GIFFORD, E. V., & WILSON, K. G. (1996). Stimulus classes and stimulus relations: Arbitrarily applicable relational responding as an operant. In T. R. ZENTALL & P. M. SMEETS (Eds.), Stimulus class formation in humans and animals (pp. 279–299). Elsevier Science B.V.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • HAYES, S. C., & HAYES, L. J. (1989). The verbal action of the listener as a basis for rule-governance. In S. C. HAYES (Ed.), Rule-governed behavior. Cognition, contingencies and instructional control (pp. 153–190). New YorK: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • HEALY O., BARNES, D., & SMEETS, P. M. (1998). Derived relational responding as an operant: The effects of between-session feedback. The Psychological Record, 48, 511–536.

    Google Scholar 

  • HEALY, O, BARNES-HOLMES, D., & SMEETS, P. M. (2000). Derived relational responding as generalized operant behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 74, 207–227.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • KOHLENBERG, B. S., HAYES, S. C., & HAYES, L. J. (1991). The transfer of contextual control over equivalence classes through equivalence classes: A possible model of social stereotyping. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 505–518.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • LANCIONI, G. E., & SMEETS, P. M. (1986). Procedures and parameters of errorless discrimination training with developmentally impaired individuals. International Review or Research in Mental Retardation, 14, 135–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LUCIANO, M. C. (1988). Un análisis de los procedimientos para la adquisiciön, eliminaciön, mantenimiento y generalizacíon del comportamiento en personas retardadas en su desarrollo [An analysis of the procedures for the acquisition, elimination, maintenance and generalization of behavior in developmentally retarded persons]. In M. C. LUCIANO & J. GIL ROALES-NIETO (Eds.), Análisis e Intervention Conductual en Retraso en el Desarrollo (pp. 51–104). Granada: Servicio de Publicaciones. Universidad de Granada.

    Google Scholar 

  • LUCIANO, M. C, HERRUZO, J., & BARNES-HOLMES, D. (2001) Generalization of say-do correspondence. The Psychological Record, 51, 111–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • LYNCH, D, & GREEN, G. (1991). Development and crossmodal transfer of contextual control of emergent stimulus relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 139–154.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • MEEHAN, E. F., & FIELDS, L. (1995). Contextual control of new equivalence classes. The Psychological Record, 45, 165–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • PEREZ, L. A. (1994). Transfer of relational stimulus control in conditional discriminations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 61, 487–503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • STEELE, D, & HAYES, S. C. (1991). Stimulus equivalence and arbitrarily applicable relational responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 519–555.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • STROMER, R., MCILVANE, W. J., & SERNA, R. W. (1993). Complex stimulus control and equivalence. The Psychological Record, 43, 585–598.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Serafín Gómez.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gómez, S., Barnes-Holmes, D. & Luciano, M.C. Generalized Break Equivalence II: Contextual Control Over A Generalized Pattern of Stimulus Relations. Psychol Rec 52, 203–220 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395424

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395424

Navigation