Skip to main content
Log in

Periodic Response-Reinforcer Contiguity: Temporal Control but not as We Know It!

  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Two experiments using rats show the effects of introducing response-reinforcer contiguity on a modified recycling conjunctive fixed-time 30 s fixed-ratio 1 schedule of food reinforcement. In Experiment 1, the frequency of programmed contiguity was varied across conditions. The general finding was an increase in overall rate of responding for all rats when the incidence of obtained cont iguity was relatively high. This increase in responding was accompanied by unusual response patterns for some rats. These patterns persisted when the rats were subsequently exposed to a fixed-interval schedule. Other rats produced fixed-interval-like performance when the incidence of obtained contiguity increased. Similar findings were observed in Experiment 2 where a more direct comparison was made between the modified recycling conjunctive schedule and the fixed-interval schedule. Results in general emphasize the importance of the modified recycling conjunctive schedule as a tool for exploring behavioral adaptation to periodic reinforcement. The history effects reported in both experiments are discussed with reference to the notion of schedule-induced behavior and temporal control.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • ANDERSON, M. C. & SHETTLEWORTH, S. J. (1977). Behavioral adaptation to fixed-interval and fixed-time food delivery in golden hamsters. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 27, 33–49.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • BARON, A., & LEINENWEBER, A. (1994). Molecular and molar analyses of fixed-interval performance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 61, 11–18.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • BARON, A., & LEINENWEBER, A. (1995). Effects of a variable-ratio conditioning history on sensitivity to fixed-interval contingencies in rats. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 63, 97–110.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • BICKEL, W. K., HIGGINS, S. T., KIRBY, K., & JOHNSTON, L. M. (1988). An inverse relationship between baseline fixed-interval response rate and the effects of a tandem response requirement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 211–218.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • BLACKMAN, D. E. (1983). On cognitive theories ot animal learning: Extrapolation from humans to animals? In G. C. L. Davey (Ed.), Animal models of human behavior. New York: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • CATANIA, A. C. (1998). Learning (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • CHURCH, R. M. (1978). The internal clock. In S. H. Hulse, H. Fowler, & W. K. Honig (Eds), Cognitive processes in animal behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • CUMMING, W. W., & SCHOENFELD, W. N. (1958). Behavior under extended exposure to a high-value fixed-interval schedule. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1, 245–263.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • DAVEY, G. (1987). Animal learning and conditioning. London: Macmillan Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • DEWS, P. B. (1970). The theory of fixed-interval responding. In W. N. Schoenfeld (Ed.), The theory of reinforcement schedules (pp. 43–61). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • FERSTER, C. B., & SKINNER, B. F. (1957). Schedules of reinforcement. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • HYTEN, C., & MADDEN, G. J. (1993). The scallop in human fixed-interval research: A review of problems with data description. The Psychological Record, 43, 471–500.

    Google Scholar 

  • JOHNSTON, L. M., BICKEL, W. K., HIGGINS, S. T., & MORRIS, E. K. (1991). The effects of schedule history and opportunity for adjunctive responding on behavior during a fixed-interval schedule of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 55, 313–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • KEENAN, M. (1982). Behavioural organisation in schedules of reinforcement. Unpublished D. Phil, thesis. University of Ulster.

    Google Scholar 

  • KEENAN, M. (1986). Second-order schedules. The Psychological Record, 36, 407–417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • KEENAN, M., & LESLIE, J. C. (1986). Varying response-reinforcer contiguity in a recycling conjunctive schedule. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 45, 317–332.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • KEENAN, M., & TOAL, L. (1991). Periodic reinforcement and second-order schedules. The Psychological Record, 41, 87–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • KEENAN, M., & WATT, A. (1990). Concurrent behavior and response reinforcer contiguity. The Psychological Record, 40, 127–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • LEINENWEBER, A., NIETZEL, S. M., & BARON, A. (1996). Temporal control by progressive-interval schedules of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 66, 311–326.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • LESLIE, J. C. (1996). Principles of behavior analysis. Amsterdam: Harwood Academic Publishers GmbH.

    Google Scholar 

  • LOWE, C. F, & WEARDEN, J. H. (1981). Weber’s law and the fixed-interval post reinforcement pause. Behavior Analysis Letters, 1, 27–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • MORSE, W. H., & KELLEHER, R. T. (1977). Determinants of reinforcement and punishment. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp. 174–200). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • RICHELLE, M., & LEJEUNE, H. (1980). Time in animal behaviour. Oxford: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • STADDON, J. E. R. (1983). Adaptive behavior and learning. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • STADDON, J. E. R. (1977). Schedule-induced behavior. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp. 125–153). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • TATHAM, T. A., WANCHISEN, B. A., & HINELINE, P. A. (1993). Effects of fixed and variable ratios on human behavioral variability. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 59, 349–359.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • THOMAS, G. (1981). Contiguity, reinforcement rate and the law of effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33B, 33–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • WANCHISEN, B. A., TATHAM, T. A., & MOONEY, S. E. (1989). Variable-ratio conditioning history produces high- and low-rate fixed-interval performance in rats. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 167–179.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • ZEILER, M. D. (1977). Schedules of reinforcement: The controlling variables. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp. 201–232). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael Keenan.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Keenan, M. Periodic Response-Reinforcer Contiguity: Temporal Control but not as We Know It!. Psychol Rec 49, 273–297 (1999). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395321

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395321

Navigation