Skip to main content
Log in

Increasing the Generativity of Identity-Based Procedures for Establishing Arbitrary Conditional Relations

  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Previous studies have shown that conditional discrimination training procedures involving identity matching to complex samples can produce emergent arbitrary relations. Identity-based training with complex stimuli therefore may be a valuable procedure for efficiently generating large networks of stimulus relations. The present study was an attempt to increase the yield of emergent relations from identity-based procedures by (a) including an extra (cross-modal) stimulus among the elements of the complex sample that was not implicated directly in the experimental contingencies and (b) including this extra stimulus in overlapping stimulus sets to promote the merger of separately trained classes. Identity-based relations were taught in the format of ABCD-X, in which Stimuli A, B, and C were visual symbols presented as a compound, D was an auditory stimulus presented simultaneously with the ABC compound, and X was a correct comparison stimulus identical to A, B, or C. Four 4-member stimulus classes emerged from this training, and pairs of classes sharing an auditory stimulus (D) subsequently merged to form two 7-member classes. The training of 12 identity-based relations produced 72 untrained arbitrary relations. This yield rivals that expected from stimulus equivalence procedures involving arbitrary-matching training and thus provides encouragement for the extension of identity-based procedures in the field of stimulus equivalence.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • BARON, A., & MENICH, S. R. (1985). Reaction times of older and younger men: Effects of compound samples and a prechoice signal on delayed matching-to-sample performances. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 44, 1–14.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • BURKE, J. C., & CERNIGLIA, L. (1990). Stimulus complexity and autistic childrens’ responsivity: Assessing and training a pivotal behavior. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 20, 233–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • CARRIGAN, P. F., & SIDMAN, M. (1992). Conditional discrimination and equivalence relations: A theoretical analysis of control by negative stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 58, 183–204.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • CRITCHFIELD, T. S., TUCKER, J. A., & VUCHINICH, R. E. (in press). Selfreport methods. In K. A. Latta I & M. Perone (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in human operant behavior. New York: Plenum.

  • DUBE, W. V., GREEN, G., & SERNA, R. W. (1993). Auditory successive conditional discrimination and auditory stimulus classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 59, 103–114.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • ERICSSON, K. A., & SIMON, H. (1984). Protocol analysis: Verbal reports as data. Cambridge, MA: Mit Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • FIELDS, L., & VERHAVE, T. (1987). The structure of equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 48, 317–332.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • GARNER, W. R. (1974). The processing of information and structure. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • HARRISON, R. J., & GREEN, G. (1990). Development of conditional and equivalence relations without differential consequences. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 54, 225–237.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • HAYES, S. C. (1986). The case of the silent dog–Verbal reports and the analysis of rules: A review of Ericsson and Simon’s Protocol Analysis: Verbal reports as data. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 45, 351–363.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • INNIS, A., LANE, S. D., MILLER, E. R., & CRITCHFIELD, T. S. (in press). Stimulus equivalence: Effects on emergent relations of a “none of the above” response option. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior.

  • JOHNSON, C., & SIDMAN, M. (1993). Conditional discrimination and equivalence relations: Control by negative stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 59, 333–347.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • LANE, S. D., & CRITCHFIELD, T. S. (1996). Verbal self-reports of emergent relations in a stimulus equivalence procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 355–374.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • LANE, S. D., & CRITCHFIELD, T. S. (1998). Classification of vowels and consonants by individuals with moderate mental retardation: Development of arbitrary relations via match-to-sample training with compound stimuli. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 21–41.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • LOVAAS, I. O., & SCHREIBMAN, L. (1971). Stimulus overselectivity of autistic children in a two-stimulus situation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 9, 305–310.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • REYNOLDS, G. S. (1961). Attention in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of behavior, 4, 203–208.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • RILEY, D. A. (1984). Do pigeons decompose stimulus compounds? In H. L. Roitblat, T. G. Bever, & H. S. Terrace (Eds.), Animal cognition (pp. 333–350). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • SAUNDERS, R. R., & GREEN, G. (1992). The nonequivalence of behavioral and mathematical equivalence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 57, 227–241.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SAUNDERS, R. R., SAUNDERS, K. J., KIRBY, K. C., & SPRADLIN, J. E. (1988). The merger and development of equivalence classes by unreinforced conditional selection of comparison stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 145–162.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SAUNDERS, R. R., SAUNDERS, K. J., WILLIAMS, D. C., & SPRADLIN, J. E. (1993). An interaction of instructions and training design on stimulus class formation: Extending the analysis of equivalence. The Psychological Record, 41, 725–744.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SAUNDERS, R. R., WACHTER, J. A., & SPRADLIN, J. E. (1988). Establishing auditory stimulus control over an eight-member equivalence class via conditional discrimination procedures. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 49, 95–115.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SCHENK, J. J. (1993). Emergent conditional discrimination in children: Matching to compound stimuli. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46B(4), 345–365.

    Google Scholar 

  • SCHENK, J. J. (1995). Complex stimuli in nonreinforced simple and conditional discriminations. The Psychological Record, 45, 477–494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M. (1994). Equivalence relations and behavior: A research story. Boston, MA: Authors Cooperative.

    Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M., KIRK, B., & WILLSON-MORRIS, M. (1985). Six-member stimulus classes generated by conditional-discrimination procedures. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43, 21–42.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M., & TAILBY, W. (1982). Conditional discrimination vs. matching to sample: An expansion of the testing paradigm. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 5–22.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SMEETS, P. M., SCHENK, J. J., & BARNES, D. (1994). Establishing transfer from identity to arbitrary matching tasks via complex stimuli under testing conditions: A follow-up study. The Psychological Record, 44, 521–536.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • STROMER, R., & MACKAY, H. A. (1992). Spelling and emergent picture-printed word relations established with delayed identity matching to complex samples. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 893–904.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • STROMER, R., & MACKAY, H. A. (1993). Delayed identity matching to complex samples: Teaching students with mental retardation spelling and the prerequisites for equivalence classes. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 14, 19–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • STROMER, R., MciLVANE, W. J., DUBE, W. V., & MACKAY, H. A. (1993). Assessing control by elements of complex stimuli in delayed matching to sample. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 59, 83–102.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • STROMER, R. MciLVANE, W. J., & SERNA, R. W. (1993). Complex stimulus control and equivalence. The Psychological Record, 43, 585–598.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • STROMER, R., & STROMER, J. B. (1990). The formation of arbitrary stimulus classes in matching to complex samples. The Psychological Record, 40, 51–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • WULFERT, E., DOUGHER, M. J., & GREENWAY, D. E. (1991). Protocol analysis of the correspondence of verbal behavior and equivalence class formation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 489–504.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • ZYGMONT, D., LAZAR, R., DUBE, W. V., & MciLVANE, W. J. (1992). Teaching arbitrary matching via sample stimulus-control shaping to young children and mentally retarded individuals. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of behavior, 57, 109–117.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Scott D. Lane.

Additional information

This research was part of a doctoral dissertation conducted by Scott D. Lane at Auburn University. Portions of the data were presented at the 1996 Association for Behavior Analysis Conference in San Francisco. Preparation of the manuscript was supported in part by a National Service Research Award to Scott Lane from the National Institutes of Health (DA05774), and by the University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston. The authors thank Robert Stromer, William Dube, and William Mcllvane for helpful comments that aided the construction of the procedures and earlier versions of this manuscript

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lane, S.D., Critchfield, T.S. Increasing the Generativity of Identity-Based Procedures for Establishing Arbitrary Conditional Relations. Psychol Rec 48, 457–479 (1998). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395284

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395284

Navigation