Skip to main content
Log in

Discrimination of “Four” and “Two” by Pigeons

  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Two pigeons learned to respond to four objects while withholding response to two objects. The birds maintained their numerical discrimination when new objects were introduced. They also maintained the discrimination when large and small objects were used to construct “2” and “4,” and when different kinds of objects were mixed to make “2” and “4.” The subjects responded most often to four objects when tested with one, two, three, four, and five objects in the generalization test after fourth discrimination task. These results suggest the ability of numerical discrimination in pigeons. Two other birds that were trained to peck two objects but not to peck four objects showed, however, severe difficulty in learning this task. Thus, pigeons’ ability for numerical discrimination is affected by training procedure.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • ARNDT, W. (1939). Abschliesende Versuche zur Frage der “Zahl” Vermoegens de Haustaube. Zeitschrift fuer Tierpsychologie, 3, 88–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • BOYSEN, S. T., & CAPALDI, E. J. (Eds.). (1993). The development of numerical competence. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlebaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • CAPALDI, E. J., MILLER, D. J., & ALPTEKIN, S. (1989). A conditional numerical discrimination based on qualitatively different reinforcer. Learning & Motivation, 20, 48–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DAVIS, H. (1984). Discrimination of the number three by a raccoon (Procyon lotor). Animal Learning and behavior, 12, 409–419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DAVIS, H., & BRADFORD, S. A. (1986). Counting behavior by rats in a simulated natural environment. Ethology, 73, 265–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DAVIS, H., & MEMMOTT, J. (1982). Counting behavior in animals: A critical evaluation. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 547–571.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DAVIS, H., & PERUSSE, R. (1988). Numerical competence in animals: Definitional issues, current evidence, and a new research agenda. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 561–615.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • JENKINS, H. M., & Sainsbury, R. S. (1970). Discrimination learning with the distinctive feature on positive or negative trials. In D. I. Mostofsky (Eds.), Attention: Contemporary theory and analysis (pp. 239–276). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • KOEHLER, O. (1941). Vom Lernen unbenannter Anzahlen bei Voegeln. Die Naturwissenschaften, 29, 201–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • KOEHLER, O. (1951). The ability of birds to “count.” Bulletin of Animal behavior, 9, 41–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • LOEGLER, P. (1959). Versuche zur Frage des Zahl-vermoegens an einem Graupapegei und Vergleichendeversuche an Menschen. Zeitschrift fuer Tierpsychologie, 16, 169–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • MAROLD, E. (1939). Versuche an Wellensittichen zur Frage des “Zahl” vermoegens. Zeitschrift fuer Tierpsychologie, 3, 170–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • PEPPERBERG, I. M. (1994). Numerical competence in an African Gray parrot (Psittacus erithacus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108, 36–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • RENSCH, B., & ALTEVOGT, R. (1957). The intelligence of elephants. Scientific American, 421–427.

    Google Scholar 

  • RILLING, M. E. (1967). Number of responses as a stimulus in fixed interval and fixed ratio schedules. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 63, 60–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • RILLING, M. E., & MCDIARMID, C. (1965). Signal detection in fixed-ratio schedule. Science, 148, 526–527.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • ROBERT, W. A., MACUDA, T., & BRODBECK, D. R. (1995). Memory for number of light flashes in the pigeons. Animal Learning and behavior, 23, 182–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SCHIEMANN, K. (1939). Vom Erlernen unbenanntes Anzahlen bei Dohlen, 3, 292–347.

    Google Scholar 

  • SEIBT, V. T. (1982). Zahlbegriff und Zaehlverhalten bei Tieren: Neue Versuche und Deutungen. Zeitschrift fuer Tierpsychologie, 60, 325–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SWENSON, L. C. (1970). One versus two discrimination by whitenecked ravens (Corvus cryptoleucus) with non-number discriminations varied. Animal behavior, 18, 454–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • WATANABE, S. (1991). Effects of ectostriatal lesions on natural concept, pseudoconcept and artificial pattern discrimination in pigeons. Visual Neuroscience, 6, 497–506.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shigeru Watanabe.

Additional information

This research was supported by Grants in Aid for Promotion of Sciences (#05206113).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Watanabe, S. Discrimination of “Four” and “Two” by Pigeons. Psychol Rec 48, 383–391 (1998). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395279

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395279

Navigation