Skip to main content
Log in

Extinction-Induced Response Variability in Humans

  • Article
  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Two experiments evaluated changes in response topography during extinction in human subjects. In Experiment 1, subjects fulfilled a sequence of DRL schedule parameters, responding on a computer keyboard to accumulate points on the monitor. Following the last DRL condition, an extinction condition was programmed during which points could no longer be accumulated. Response/reinforcer ratios increased consistently with each change in schedule parameter and interresponse time (IRT) distributions during extinction showed increased variability relative to preextinction baseline responding. In Experiment 2, subjects responded on a single DRL schedule value prior to being placed on extinction. Results paralleled those of Experiment 1, indicating greater response variability during extinction than during the DRL phase. The results are discussed within the context of a selectionist perspective on operant behavior, with extinction being identified as one ontogenic source of behavioral variability.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • ANTONITIS, J. J. (1951). Response variability in the white rat during conditioning, extinction and reconditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 42, 273–281.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • AZRIN, N. H., HUTCHINSON, R. R., & HAKE, D. F. (1966). Extinction-induced aggression. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9, 191–204.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • BARASH, D. R (1982). Sociobiology and behavior. New York: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • BOREN, J. J., & DEVINE, D. D. (1968). The repeated acquisition of behavioral chains. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11, 651–660.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • CAMPBELL, D. T. (1987). Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. In G. Radnitzky & W.W. Bartley, III (Eds.), Evolutionary epistemology rationality and the sociology of knowledge. La Salle, IL: Open Court.

    Google Scholar 

  • CATANIA, A. C., & HARNARD, S. (Eds.) (1986). The selection of behavior. The operant behaviorism of B. F. Skinner: Comments and consequences. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • COSMIDES, L., & TOOBY, J. (1989). Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture, Part 1: Theoretical considerations. Ethology and Sociobiology, 10, 29–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • CRAWFORD, C. B. (1989). The theory of evolution: Of what value to psychology? Comparative Psychology, 103, 4–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • CRAWFORD, C. B., & ANDERSON, J. L. (1989). Sociobiology: An environmental discipline? American Psychologist, 44, 1449–1459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DONAHOE, J. W., BURGOS, J. E., & PALMER, D. C. (1993). Selectionist approach to reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 58, 17–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DONAHOE, J. W., & PALMER, D. C. (1994). Learning and complex behavior. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • EPSTEIN, R. (1983). Resurgence of previously reinforced behavior during extinction. Behavior Analysis Letters, 3, 391–397.

    Google Scholar 

  • EPSTEIN, R. (1985). Extinction-induced resurgence: Preliminary investigations and possible applications. The Psychological Record, 35, 143–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • FERSTER, C. B., & SKINNER, B. F. (1957). Schedules of reinforcement New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • GRUBER, H. E., & DAVIS, S. N. (1988). Inching our way up Mount Olympus: The evolving systems approach to creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The nature of creativity: Contemporary psychological perspectives (pp. 243–270). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • HARLOW, H. F. (1949). The formation of learning sets. Psychological Review, 56, 51–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • HONIG, W. K., & STADDON, J. E. R. (1977). Handbook of operant behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • HUMPHREYS, L. G. (1939). Acquisition and extinction of verbal expectations in a situation analogous to conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25, 294–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • JOHNSTON, J. M., & PENNYPACKER, H. S. (1993). Strategies and tactics of behavioral research. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • KOHLER, W. (1925). The mentality of apes. New York: Harcourt, Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • LOVAAS, I. O., & SIMMONS, J. Q. (1969). Manipulation of self-destruction in three retarded children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 2, 143–157.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • MACHADO, A. (1989). Operant conditioning of behavioral variability using a percentile reinforcement schedule. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 155–166.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • NEISWORTH, J. T., & MOORE, F. (1972). Operant treatment of asthmatic responding with the parent as therapist. Behavior Therapy, 3, 95–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • NEWELL, A., & SIMON, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NY: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • PAGE, S., & NEURINGER, A. (1985). Variability is an operant. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 11, 429–452.

    Google Scholar 

  • PERKINS, D. N. (1988). The possibility of invention. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The nature of creativity: Contemporary psychological perspectives (pp. 362–385). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • PRYOR, K. W., HAAG, R., & O’REILLY, J. (1969). The creative porpoise: Training for novel behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 653–661.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • RICHELLE, M. (1987). Variation and selection: The evolutionary analogy in Skinner’s theory. In S. Modgil & C. Modgil (Eds.), B. F. Skinner: Consensus and controversy. New York: Falmer Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • RILLING, M. (1977). Stimulus control and inhibitory processes. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp. 432–480). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • RINGEN, J. D. (1993). Adaptation, teleology, and selection by consequences. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 60, 3–15.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • ROBBINS, D. (1971). Partial reinforcement: A selective review of the alleyway literature since 1960. Psychological Bulletin, 76, 415–431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SCHOENFELD, W. N., HARRIS, A. H., & FARMER, J. (1966). Conditioning response variability. Psychological Reports, 19, 551–557.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • SCHWARTZ, B. (1980). Development of complex stereotyped behavior in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 33, 153–166.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SCHWARTZ, B. (1982). Failure to produce response variability with reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 171–181.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SKINNER, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • SKINNER, B. F. (1966). The ontogeny and phylogeny of behavior. Science, 153, 1203–1213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SKINNER, B. F. (1981). Selection by consequences. Science, 213, 501–504.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • STAATS, A. W. (1981). Paradigmatic behaviorism, unified theory construction, and the Zeitgeist of separatism. American Psychologist, 36, 239–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SYMONS, D. (1987). If we’re all Darwinians, what’s the fuss about? In C. B. Crawford, M. S. Smith, & D. Krebs (Eds.), Sociobiology and psychology: Ideas, issues, and applications (pp. 121–146). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • TODD, J. T., MORRIS, E. K., & FENZA, K. M. (1989). Temporal organization of extinction-induced responding in preschool children. The Psychological Record, 39, 117–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • VOGEL, R., & ANNAU, Z. (1973). An operant discrimination task allowing variability of response patterning. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 20, 1–6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • WEINER, H. (1965). Conditioning history and maladaptive human operant behavior. Psychological Reports, 17, 935–942.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • WEINER, H. (1969). Controlling human fixed-interval performance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 349–373.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • WEINER, H. (1972). Controlling human fixed-interval performance with fixed-ratio responding or differential reinforcement of low-rate responding in mixed schedules. Psychonomic Science, 26, 191–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Morgan, D.L., Lee, K. Extinction-Induced Response Variability in Humans. Psychol Rec 46, 145–159 (1996). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395168

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395168

Navigation