Skip to main content
Log in

A Transfer of Explicitly and Nonexplicitly Trained Sequence Responses Through Equivalence Relations: An Experimental Demonstration and Connectionist Model

  • Article
  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Eight college students were trained in a sequential response to two nonsense syllable pairs B1–B2 and B2–B3. They were then tested to determine whether an untrained, novel three-response sequence emerged as a result of the spontaneous combination of the two explicitly trained two-response sequences (i.e., B1–B2 + B2–B3 becomes B1–B2–B3). All subjects demonstrated this three-response sequence performance. Four experimental subjects were then trained in a set of conditional discriminations, using a matching-to-sample procedure (i.e., A1–B1, A1–C1, A2–B2, A2–C2, A3–B3, A3–C3). All four subsequently displayed the formation of three equivalence relations in nonreinforced testing (i.e., A1–B1–C1, A2–B2–C2, A3–B3–C3). This was followed by testing for a transfer, through equivalence, of the sequential responses, for both nonsense syllable pairs and triples. All experimental subjects showed this transfer (i.e., C1–C2, C2–C3, and C1–C2-C3). The control subjects, however, who had not received the conditional discrimination training and equivalence testing, all failed to show this transfer. A connectionist model was then designed to simulate the obtained data. After suitable explicit training on up to four stimulus sets, the model was capable of correctly responding to the sequence, equivalence, and transfer tests, after exposure to only the training stages of a fifth set. The wider implications of connectionism for behavior analysis are also considered.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • BARNES, D. (1994). Stimulus equivalence and rational frame theory. The Psychological Record, 44, 91–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • BARNES, D., & HAMPSON, P. J. (1993a). Learning to learn: The contribution of behaviour analysis to connectionist models of inferential skill in humans. In Neural Computing Research and Applications: Part One (pp. 129–138). Iop, England.

    Google Scholar 

  • BARNES, D., & HAMPSON, P. J. (1993b). Stimulus equivalence and connectionism: Implications for behavior analysis and cognitive science. The Psychological Record, 43, 617–638.

    Google Scholar 

  • BARNES, D., & HOLMES, Y. (1991). Radical behaviorism, stimulus equivalence, and human cognition. The Psychological Record, 41, 19–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • BARNES, D., MCCULLAGH, P. D., & KEENAN, M. (1990). Equivalence class formation in non-hearing impaired children and hearing impaired children. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 8, 19–30.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • CHOMSKY, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • CHOMSKY, N. (1959). Review of Skinner’s Verbal Behavior. Language, 35, 26–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • DEVANY, J. M., HAYES, S. C., & NELSON, R. O. (1986). Equivalence class formation in language-able and language-disabled children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 46, 243–257.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • DONAHOE, J. W., BURGOES, J. E., & PALMER, C. P. (1993). A selectionist approach to reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 60, 17–40.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • DUGDALE, N., & LOWE, C. F. (1990). Naming and stimulus equivalence. In D. Blackman & H. Lejeune (Eds.), Behaviour analysis in theory and practice (pp. 115–138). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • EYSENCK, M.W., & KEANE, M. T. (1990). Cognitive psychology: A students’ handbook. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • HAYES, S.C. (1989). Nonhumans have not yet shown stimulus equivalence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 385–392.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • HAYES, S. C. (1991). A relational control theory of stimulus equivalence. In L. J. Hayes & P. N. Chase (Eds.), Dialogues of verbal behavior: The first international institute on verbal relations (pp. 19–40). Reno, NV: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • HAYES, S. C., & HAYES, L. J. (1992). Verbal relations and the evolution of behavior analysis. American Psychologist, 47, No. 11. 1383–1395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • HAYES, S. C., KOHLENBERG, B. S., & HAYES, L. J. (1991). The transfer of specific and general consequential functions through simple and conditional equivalence relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 119–137.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • HETHERINGTON, P. A., & SEIDERBERG, M. S. (1989). Is there “catastrophic interference” in connectionist networks? Proceedings of the Eleventh Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 173–178). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • LAZAR, R. (1977). Extending sequence-class membership with matching to sample. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 27. 381–392.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • LAZAR, R. M., & KOTLARCHYK, B. J. (1986). Second-order control of sequence-class equivalences in children. Behavioral Processes, 13, 205–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MACKAY, H. A., & SIDMAN, M. (1984). Teaching new behaviors via equivalence relations. In P. Brooks, R. Sperber, & C. McCauley (Eds.), Learning and cognition in the mentally retarded (pp. 493–513). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • MCCLELLAND, J. L. (1981). Retrieving general and specific information from stored knowledge of specifics. Proceedings of the Third Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 170–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • MCCLELLAND, J. L., & RUMELHART, D. E. (1988). Explorations in parallel distributed processing, A handbook of models, programs, and exercises. London England: Mit Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • RUMMELHART, D. E., HINTON, G. E., & WILLIAMS, R. J. (1986). Learning representations by back-propagating errors. Nature, 323, 533–536.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SCHUSTERMAN, R. J., & KASTAK, D. (1993). A california sea lion (Zalophus californianus) is capable of forming equivalence relations. The Psychological Record, 43, 823–839.

    Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M. (1986). Functional analysis of emergent verbal classes. In T. Thompson & M. D. Zeiler (Eds.), Analysis and Integration of behavioral units. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M.(1990). Equivalence relations: Where do they come from? In D. E. Blackman & H. Lejeune (Eds.), Behaviour analysis in theory and practice: Contributions and controversies (pp. 92–114). Hove, England: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M. (1992). Equivalence relations: Some basic considerations. In Steven C. Hayes & Linda J. Hayes (Eds.), Understanding verbal relations (pp. 15–27). Reno, NV: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M., RAUZIN, R., LAZAR, R., CUNNINGHAM, S., TAILBY, W., & CARRIGAN, P. (1982). A search for symmetry in the conditional discriminations of rhesus monkeys, baboons and children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43, 21–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SKINNER, B. F. (1974). About behaviorism. London, England: Jonathan Cape.

    Google Scholar 

  • STEELE, D. M., & HAYES, S. C. (1991). Stimulus equivalence and arbitrarily applicable relational responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 519–555.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • STROMER, R., MCILVANE, W. J., & SERNA, R. W. (1993). Complex stimulus control and equivalence. The Psychological Record, 43, 585–598.

    Google Scholar 

  • WULFERT, E., & HAYES, S. C. (1988). Transfer of a conditional ordering response through conditional equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 125–144.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Cullinan, V.A., Barnes, D., Hampson, P.J. et al. A Transfer of Explicitly and Nonexplicitly Trained Sequence Responses Through Equivalence Relations: An Experimental Demonstration and Connectionist Model. Psychol Rec 44, 559–585 (1994). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395144

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395144

Navigation