Skip to main content
Log in

The Spontaneous Interconnection of Three Repertoires

  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A previous study (Epstein, Kirshnit, Lanza, & Rubin, 1984) showed that pigeons that had acquired two relevant behaviors (pushing a box toward targets, and climbing onto a box and then pecking a small facsimile of a banana) could solve the classic box-and-banana problem. A human-like solution emerged as a result of the manner in which the two repertoires became interconnected moment-to-moment in time (Epstein et al., 1984; Epstein & Medalie, 1983). In the current experiment, a pigeon acquired three separate behaviors: (a) climbing, (b) pushing toward targets, and (c) pecking the banana. When the pigeon was confronted with the problem, a swift but erratic and not especially “insightful” solution emerged. Some simple principles shed light on the differences between the performances generated by the interconnection of two repertoires and the interconnection of three repertoires in this situation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • CUMMING, W. W., & ECKERMAN, D. A. (1965). Stimulus control of a differentiated operant. Psychonomic Science, 3, 313–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DASHIELL, J. F. (1930). Direction orientation in maze running by the white rat. Comparative Psychology Monographs, 7.

    Google Scholar 

  • DUNCKER, K. (1945). On problem-solving. Psychological Monographs, No. 270.

    Google Scholar 

  • ELLEN, P. (1982). Direction, past experience, and hints in creative problem solving: Reply to Weisberg and Alba. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 111, 316–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • EPSTEIN, R. (1981). On pigeons and people: A preliminary look at the Columban Simulation Project. The Behavior Analyst, 4(1), 43–55.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • EPSTEIN, R. (1983). Resurgence of previously reinforced behavior during extinction. Behaviour Analysis Letters, 3, 391–397.

    Google Scholar 

  • EPSTEIN, R. (1984a). Simulation research in the analysis of behavior. Behaviorism, 12(2), 41–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • EPSTEIN, R. (1984b, September). A moment-to-moment account of the emergence of a novel performance. Invited address given at the meeting of the International Society for Comparative Psychology, Acapulco.

    Google Scholar 

  • EPSTEIN, R. (1985). Extinction-induced resurgence: Preliminary investigations and possible applications. The Psychological Record, 35, 131–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • EPSTEIN, R. (in press). Bringing cognition and creativity into the behavioral laboratory. In T. J. Knapp & L. Robertson (Eds.), Approaches to cognition: Contrasts and controversies. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

  • EPSTEIN, R., KIRSHNIT, C., LANZA, R., & RUBIN, L. (1984). ‘Insight’ in the pigeon: Antecedents and determinants of an intelligent performance. Nature, 308, 1 March, 61–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • EPSTEIN, R., LANZA, R. P., & SKINNER, B. F. (1980). Symbolic communication between two pigeons (Columba livia domestica). Science, 207, 543–545.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • EPSTEIN, R., & MEDALIE, S. (1983). The spontaneous use of a tool by a pigeon. Behaviour Analysis Letters, 3, 241–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • EPSTEIN, R., & SKINNER, B. F. (1981). The spontaneous use of a memorandum by pigeons. Behaviour Analysis Letters, 1, 241–246.

    Google Scholar 

  • HULL, C. L. (1935). The mechanism of the assembly of behavior segments in novel combinations suitable for problem solving. Psychological Review, 42, 219–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • HULL, C. L. (1952). A behavior system. New Haven, Ct: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • KÖHLER, W. (1925). The mentality of apes. New York: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • KOFFKA, K. (1924). The growth of the mind. London: Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • MAIER, N. R. F. (1929). Reasoning in white rats. Comparative Psychology Monographs, 6.

    Google Scholar 

  • MAIER, N. R. F. (1931). Reasoning and learning. Psychological Review, 38, 332–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MAIER, N. R. F. (1932). The effect of cortical destruction on reasoning and learning in white rats. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 54, 45–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MIGLER, B. (1964). Effects of averaging data during stimulus generalization. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 7, 303–307.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • YERKES, R. M. (1929). The great apes. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

The work was supported in part by NIH Grant MH32628 to the Foundation for Research on the Nervous System.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Epstein, R. The Spontaneous Interconnection of Three Repertoires. Psychol Rec 35, 131–141 (1985). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03394917

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03394917

Navigation