Skip to main content
Log in

California Sea Lions Are Capable of Semantic Comprehension

  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Two California sea lions (a 6-year-old female, Rocky, and a 3-year-old male, Bucky) were trained in a symbolic matching paradigm. Gestural signs produced by movements of the trainer’s arms and hands referred to types of objects, modifiers, and actions. Both sea lions were eventually trained with three-sign constructions consisting of Modifier + Object + Action in that order. An example of a three-sign sequence is Black Ball Mouth [glossed as “go over to the black ball (and not the gray or white balls or the black, white, and gray pipes) and place your open mouth on it]. Rocky’s modifiers consisted of size and color attributes, and Bucky’s modifiers consisted of different locations of the objects.

After 24 months of training, Rocky had a comprehension vocabulary of 20 signs (5 modifiers, 10 objects, and 5 actions), and after 20 months of training, Bucky had a comprehension vocabulary of 16 signs (2 modifiers, 8 objects, and 6 actions). Semantic comprehension is considered to be demonstrated if an organism can follow directions when each sign is contrasted with every other in a series of commands. Currently, Rocky’s semantic comprehension consists of 190 three-sign combinations, and Bucky’s semantic comprehension consists of 64 three-sign combinations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • BLOUGH, D. S. (1959). Delayed matching in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2, 151–160.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • BLOUGH, D. S., & BLOUGH, P. (1977). Animal psychophysics. In W. Honig. & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • CARTER, D. E., & WERNER, T. J. (1978). Complex learning and informational processing by pigeons: A critical analysis. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 29, 565–601.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • CHENEY, D. L., & SEYFARTH, R. M. (1982). How vervet monkeys perceive their grunts: Field playback experiments. Animal Behaviour, 30, 739–751.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • CONSTANTINE, B. (1981, December). Matching to sample in harbor seals. Paper presented at the Fourth Biennial Conference on the Biology of Marine Mammals, San Francisco, California.

    Google Scholar 

  • ECKERMAN, D. A., LANSON, R. N., & CUMMING, J. W. (1968). Acquisition and maintenance without a required observing response. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11, 435–441.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • EPSTEIN, R., & SKINNER, B. F. (1981). The spontaneous use of memoranda by pigeons. Behavior Analysis Letters, 1, 241–246.

    Google Scholar 

  • ERRINGTON, P. L. (1946). Predation and vertebrate populations. Quarterly Review of Biology, 21, 221–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GARDNER, B. T., & GARDNER, R. A. (1969). Teaching sign language to a chimpanzee. Science, 165, 664–672.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • GRIFFIN, D. R. (1981). The question of animal awareness. New York: Rockefeller University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • HERMAN, L. M. (1980). Cognitive characteristics of dolphins. In L. M. Herman (Ed.), Cetacean behavior: Mechanisms and function. New York: John Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • ILERMAN, L. M., RICHARDS, D. G., & WOLZ, J. P. (1981, December). Can the bottlenosed dolphin understand a sentence? Paper presented at the Fourth Biennial Conference on the Biology of Marine Mammals, San Francisco, California.

    Google Scholar 

  • LASHLEY, K. S. (1938). Conditional reactions in the rat. Journal of Psychology, 6, 311–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MENZEL, E. W. (1978). Cognitive mapping in chimpanzees. In S. Hulse, H. Fowler, & W. Honig (Eds.), Cognitive aspects of animal behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • MUELLER, H. C. (1971). Oddity and specific search image more important than conspicuousness in prey selection. Nature, 233, 345–346.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • PEPPERBERG, I. M. (1981). Functional vocalizations by an African gray parrot. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie, 55, 139–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • PREMACK, A. (1976). Why chimps can read. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • PREMACK, D. (1983). The codes of man and beasts. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 6, 125–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • RIOPELLE, A. (1960). Complex processes. In R. H. Waters, D. A. Rethlingshafer, & W. E. Caldwell (Eds.), Principles of comparative psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • ROITBLAT, H. L. (1982). The meaning of representation in animal memory. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 5, 353–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • RUMBAUGH, D. M. (Ed.). (1977). Language learning in a chimpanzee: The Lana Project. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • SAVAGE-RUMBAUGH, E. S. (1981). Can apes use symbols to represent their world? In T. A. Sebeak & R. Rosenthal (Eds.), The Clever Hans phenomenon: Communication with horses, whales, apes and people. New York:: N.Y. Academy Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • SCHUSTERMAN, R. J. (1962). Transfer effects of successive discrimination-reversal training in chimpanzees. Science, 137, 422–423.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • SCHUSTERMAN, R. J. (1980). Behavioral methodology in echolocation by marine mammals. In R.-G. Busnel & J. F. Fish (Eds.), Animal sonar systems. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • SCHUSTERMAN, R. J., SILVA, R., NAVID, A., BRICK, M., & NEKLASON, S. (1981, December). Preliminary studies of California sea lions gestural language comprehension: Phas. 1. “Verb” or action training. Paper presented at the Fourth Biennial Conference on the Biology of Marine Mammals, San Francisco, California.

    Google Scholar 

  • SEYFARTH, R. M., CHENEY, D. L., & MARLER, P. (1980). Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: Evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. Science, 210, 801–803.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • SKINNER, B. F. (1969). Contingencies of reinforcement. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • SOLOMON, R. L. (1981). Prologue: Reminiscences. In N. E. Spear & R. R. Miller (Eds.), Information processing in animals: Memory mechanisms. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • TERRACE, H. S. (1982). Animal versus human minds. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 5, 391–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • WILLARD, M. J., DANA, K., STARK, L., OWEN, J., ZAZULA, J., & CORCORAN, P. (1982). Training a capuchin (cebus apella) to perform as an aide for a quadriplegic. Primates, 23, 520–532.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

This paper is dedicated to the memory of Winthrop N. Kellogg, whose truly creative and dedicated work on the neural mechanisms of learning, the developing mind of a home-raised chimpanzee, and the echolocation capabilities of dolphins continues to be an inspiration to contemporary comparative psycholgists.

We recognize the support of the Office of Naval Research Contract N00014-77-C-0185 and the Research Institute of Marine World/Africa Usa. Next, we gratefully acknowledge the help of many student volunteers who worked so diligently in maintaining the sea lions and conducting the research. Special thanks to Christine Johnson, Brigit Grimm, Renee Butler, Alex Philipoff, Chris Stone, Marianne Brick, Sharon Neklason, Hilary Holt, and Barbara Beard. Thanks also to the trainers at Marine World/Africa Usa for looking after Bucky during our absence. Finally, we want to thank Drs. Louis Herman, Jim Wolz, and Doug Richards for their encouragement and wisdom in helping to get this project started.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Schusterman, R.J., Krieger, K. California Sea Lions Are Capable of Semantic Comprehension. Psychol Rec 34, 3–23 (1984). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03394849

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03394849

Navigation