Skip to main content
Log in

Whether, when, and why Skinner published on biological participation in behavior

  • Published:
The Behavior Analyst Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper brings some data to bear on the criticisms, claims, and arguments that Skinner (a) denied or dismissed biological participation in behavior, (b) addressed it only late in his career or more often later than earlier, or (c) addressed it only because of the overwhelming evidence for it or the criticisms that he had overlooked it. For this, we coded Skinner’s every primary-source publication for three content categories (i.e., genetics, physiology, and evolution) and for the extent to which he addressed them (i.e., in publication titles, substantively, or in passing). Our findings are that Skinner addressed biological participation in over a third of his publications throughout his career, albeit more often later than earlier. The latter, however, is accounted for by an increase in his base rate of publication and in general conditions and specific events in his career, psychology, and science. The discussion addresses our research methods; the reasons for and refutations of the criticisms, claims, and arguments; and their sources.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Andresen, J. T. (1990). Skinner and Chomsky thirty years later: Or, the return of the repressed. Historiographica Linguistica, 17, 145–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. J. (1986). The cognitive revolution in psychology. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baerends, G. P. (1984). Ontogenetic or phylogenetic—another afterpain of the fallacious Cartesian dichotomy. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 679–680.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bailey, R. E., & Bailey, M. B. (1980). A view from outside the Skinner box. American Psychologist, 35, 942–946.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barash, D. P. (1984). Contingencies of selection, reinforcement, and survival. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 680.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barkow, J. H. (1984). Of false dichotomies and larger frames. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 680–681.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnes, T. C, & Skinner, B. F. (1930). The progressive increase in the geotropic response of the ant Aphaenogaster. Journal of General Psychology, 4, 102–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Black, M. (1973). Some aversive responses to a would-be reinforcer. In H. Wheeler (Ed.), Beyond the punitive society: Operant conditioning: Social and political aspects (pp. 125–134). San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blanchard, D. C, Blanchard, R. J., & Flannelly, K. J. (1984). A new experimental analysis of behavior—one for all behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 681–682.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breland, K., & Breland, M. (1961). The misbehavior of organisms. American Psychologist, 16, 681–684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burghardt, G. M. (1984). Ethology and operant psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 683–684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catania, A. C. (1991). The gifts of culture and of eloquence: An open letter to Michael Mahoney in reply to his article, “Scientific psychology and radical behaviorism.” The Behavior Analyst, 14, 61–72.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1959). Review of Skinner’s Verbal Behavior. Language, 35, 26–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Churchland, P. S. (1986). Neurophilosophy: Toward a unified science of the mind brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, I. B. (1985). Revolution in science. Cambridge, MA: Belknap.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, S. R. (1982). B. F Skinner: Systematic iconoclast. The Gamut, 6, 53–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, S. R., & Mehlman, S. E. (1992). An empirical update (1969–1989) of D. L. Krantz’s thesis that the experimental analysis of behavior is isolated. The Behavior Analyst, 15, 43–49.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Czubaroff, J. (1988). Criticism and response in the Skinner controversies. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 49, 321–329.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. R. (1859). The origins of species. London: Murray.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. R. (1871). The descent of man. London: Murray.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F (1999). The end of nature versus nurture. Scientific American, 281, 94–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F B. M. (2001 ). The ape and the sushi master: Cultural reflections of a primatologist. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F (2004, May). Brains and the beast. Natural History, pp. 53–56.

  • Delius, J. D. (1984). Consequence contingencies and provenance partitions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 685.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dewsbury, D. A. (1984). Comparative psychology in the twentieth century. Stroudsburg, PA: Hutchinson Ross.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dewsbury, D. A. (1996). Paul Harkai Schiller: The influence of his brief career. In G. A. Kimble, C. A. Boneau, & M. Wertheimer (Eds.), Portraits of pioneers in psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 281–293). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dobzhansky, T. G. (1937). Genetics and the origin of species. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, R., & Olson, J. K. (1984). An index to B. F Skinner’s The Shaping of a Behaviorist. The Behavior Analyst, 7, 47–63.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, R., & Olson, J. K. (1985). An index to B. F Skinner’s A Matter of Consequences. The Behavior Analyst, 8, 209–233.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fearing, F (1930). A study in the history of physiological psychology. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F (1957). Schedules of reinforcement. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Garcia, J. (1981). Tilting at the paper mills of academe. American Psychologist, 36, 149–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garcia, J. (1993). Misrepresentation of my criticisms of Skinner. American Psychologist, 48, 1158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garcia, J., & Garcia, y, Robertson, R. (1985). Evolution of learning mechanisms. In B. L. Hammonds (Ed.), The Master Lecture Series (pp. 191–243). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottlieb, G. (1997). Synthesizing nature-nurture: Prenatal roots of instinctive behavior. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottlieb, G. (1999). Probabilistic epigenesis and evolution. Worchester, MA: Clark University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould, J. L. (1982). Ethology: The mechanisms and evolution of behavior. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould, J. L., & Marier, P. (1987). Learning by instinct. Scientific American, 256, 74–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heron, W T, & Skinner, B. F (1937). The effects of certain drugs and hormones on conditioning and extinction [Abstract]. Psychological Bulletin, 34, 741–742.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heron, W T, & Skinner, B. F (1939). The rate of extinction in maze-bright and maze-dull rats [Abstract]. Psychological Bulletin, 36, 520.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heron, W T, & Skinner, B. F (1940). The rate of extinction in maze-bright and maze-dull rats. The Psychological Record, 4, 11–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinde, R. A., & Stevenson-Hinde, J. (Eds.). (1973). Constraints on learning: Limitations and predispositions. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huxley, J. (1942). Evolution, the modern synthesis. New York: Harper.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, T. D. (1982). Learning the evolution of developmental systems. In H. C. Plotkin (Ed.), Learning, development, and culture (pp. 411–441). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knapp, T. J. (1974). An index to B. F Skinner’s Beyond Freedom and Dignity. Behaviorism, 2, 180–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knapp, T. J. (1975). An index to B. F. Skinner’s Waiden Two. Behaviorism, 3, 222–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krantz, D. L. (1972). Schools and systems: The mutual isolation of operant and nonoperant psychology as a case study. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 8, 86–102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lachman, R., Lachman, J., & Butterfield, E. (1979). Cognitive psychology and information processing. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lambert, E. E, Skinner, B. F, & Forbes, A. (1933). Some conditions affecting intensity and duration thresholds in motor nerve, with reference to Chronaxie of subordination. American Journal of Physiology, 106, 721 –737.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazo, J. F, Morris, E. K., & Smith, N. G. (2002, May). When Skinner said what about biological participation. In E. K. Morris (Chair), Studies in Skinner’s science and system. A symposium conducted at the meeting of the Association for Behavior Analysis, Toronto, Ontario.

  • Leahey, T. H. (2001). A history of modern psychology (3rd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, V. L. (1989). Comments about the isolation of behavior analysis. The Behavior Analyst, 12, 85–87.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lehrman, D. S. (1953). A critique of Konrad Lorenz’s theory of instinctive behavior. Quarterly Review of Biology, 28, 247–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. (1950). The comparative method of studying innate behavior patterns. In J. E Danielli & R. Brown (Eds.), Physiological mechanisms in animal behavior (pp. 221–268). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. (1952). King Solomon’s ring. New York: Thomas Y Crowell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. (1961). Phylogenetische anpassung und adaptive modification des Verhaltens. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 18, 139–187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. (1965). Evolution and modification of behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lubek, I., & Apfelbaum, E. (1987). Neo-behaviorism and the Garcia effects: A social psychology of science approach to the history of a paradigm clash. In M. G. Ash & W. R. Woodward (Eds.), Psychology in twentieth century thought and society (pp. 59–91). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mahoney, M. J. (1989). Scientific psychology and radical behaviorism. American Psychologist, 44, 1372–1377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, E. (1942). Systematics and the origin of species from the viewpoint of a zoologist. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Medawar, P. B. (1977, October 25). Fear and DNA. The New York Review of Books, pp. 15–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Midgley, B. D., & Morris, E. K. (1992). Nature = f(nurture). A review of Oyama’s The Ontogeny of Information. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 58, 229–240.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, B. R., & Stuttard, S. (1979). Dr. Guthrie and Felis domesticus or tripping over the cat. Science, 205, 1031–1033.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, J. (2002). Some thoughts on the relation between behavior analysis and neuroscience. The Psychological Record, 52, 261–279.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morris, E. K., Lazo, J. F, & Smith, N. G. (2004, August). Whether, when, and why Skinner published on biological participation. In E. K. Morris (Chair), Behaviorism and interaction with biological themes. Paper session conducted at the meeting of the International Conference of the Brazilian Association for Psychotherapy and Behavioral Medicine and the Association for Behavior Analysis, Campinas, Brazil.

  • Morris, E. K., & Smith, N. G. (2003). The process and product of bibliography, and a bibliography of the published primary-source works of B. F. Skinner. The Behavior Analyst, 26, 41–67.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Oyama, S. (1985). The ontogeny of information. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oyama, S., Griffiths, P. E., & Gray, R. D. (Eds.). (2001). Cycles of contingency: Developmental systems and evolution. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parkins, P. V. (1966). BioSciences Information Service of biological abstracts. Science, 152, 889–894.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (2002). The blank slate: The modern denial of human nature. New York: Viking.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plotkin, H. (1987). The evolutionary analogy in Skinner’s writings. In S. Modgil & C. Modgil (Eds.), B. F. Skinner: Consensus and controversy (pp. 139–149). New York: Falmer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, R. A. (2001). Public intellectuals: A study of decline. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, R. W, & Weeks, D. J. (1990a). The goal of B. F. Skinner and behavior analysis. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, R. W, & Weeks, D. J. (1990b). There is no room for scientism in scientific psychology: A comment on Mahoney. American Psychologist, 45, 1177–1178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reese, H. W. (1995). How is physiology relevant to behavior analysis? The Behavior Analyst, 19, 61–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, G. S., Catania, A. C, & Skinner, B. F (1963). Conditioned and unconditioned aggression in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 6, 73–74.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Richelle, M. (1987). The evolutionary analogy in Skinner’s theory. In S. Modgil & C. Modgil (Eds.), B. F. Skinner: Consensus and controversy (pp. 127–137). New York: Falmer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaal, D. W (2003). Explanatory reductionism in behavior analysis. In K. A. Lattal & P. N. Chase (Eds.), Behavior theory and philosophy (pp. 83–102). New York: Kluwer Academic/ Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Schiller, P. H. (1952). Innate constituents of complex responses in primates. Psychological Review, 59, 177–191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schiller, P. H. (1957). Innate motor action as a basis for learning. In C. H. Schiller (Ed.), Instinctive behavior: The development of a modern concept (pp. 264–287). New York: International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, S. M. (2003). Evolution, behavior principles, and developmental systems: A review of Gottlieb’s Synthesizing Nature-Nurture: Prenatal Roots of Instinctive Behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 79, 137–152.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. P. E., & Hagar, J. L. (Eds.). (1972). Biological boundaries of learning. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherrard, C. (1988). Rhetorical weapons: Chomsky’s attack on Skinner. Educational Psychology, 8, 197–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shettleworth, S. J. (1972). Constraints on learning. In D. S. Lehrman, R. A. Hinde, & E. Shaw (Eds.), Advances in the study of behavior (Vol. 4, pp. 1–68). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1930a). On the conditions of elicitation of certain eating reflexes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 16, 433–438.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1930b). On the inheritance of maze behavior. Journal of General Psychology, 5, 427–458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1931). The concept of the reflex in the description of behavior. Journal of General Psychology, 5, 427–458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1932a). Drive and reflex strength. Journal of General Psychology, 6, 22–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1932b). Drive and reflex strength: II. Journal of General Psychology, 6, 38–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: An experimental analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1947). Experimental psychology. In W. Dennis (Ed.), Current trends in psychology (pp. 16–49). Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1948). Waiden two. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1950). Are theories of learning necessary? Psychological Review, 57, 193–216.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. E (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1956a). A case study in scientific behavior. American Psychologist, 11, 221–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1956b). What is psychotic behavior? In F. Gildea (Ed.), Theory and treatment of the psychoses: Some newer aspects (pp. 77–99). St. Louis, MO: Committee on Publications, St. Louis University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. E (1958). Review of the book The Pigeon. Science, 127, 695.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1959a). Animal research in the pharmacotherapy of mental disease. In J. Cole & R. Gerard (Eds.), Psychopharmacology: Problems in evaluation (pp. 224–228). Washington, DC: National Academy of Sciences, National Research Council.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1959b). Cumulative record. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1959c). John B. Watson, behaviorist. Science, 129, 197–198.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1961a). Cumulative record (Enlarged ed.). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1961b). Teaching machines. Scientific American, 205, 90–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1964a). “Man.” Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 108, 482–485.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. E (1964b). New methods and new aims in teaching. New Scientist, 122, 483–484.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1966a). The phylogeny and ontogeny of behavior. Science, 153, 1205–1213.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1966b). What is the experimental analysis of behavior? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9, 213–218.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1967). B. E Skinner. In E. G. Boring & G. Lindzey (Eds.), A history of psychology in autobiography (Vol. 5, pp. 387–413). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1968). The technology of teaching. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1969a). Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1969b). The inside story. In B. E Skinner, Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical analysis (pp. 269–297). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1971a). Autoshaping. Science, 173, 752.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1971b). A behavioral analysis of value judgments. In E. Tobach, L. R. Aronson, & E. Shaw (Eds.), The biopsychology of development (pp. 543–551). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1971c). Beyond freedom and dignity. New York: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1971d, August). Beyond freedom and dignity. Psychology Today, 5, 37–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1972a). Cumulative record (3rd ed.). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. E (1972b). Humanism and behaviorism. The Humanist, 32, 18–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1973a). Answers for my critics. In H. Wheeler (Ed.), Beyond the punitive society (pp. 256–266). San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. E (1973b). Are we to have a future? Impact, 3, 6–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. E (1973c). Humanism and behaviorism. In P. Kurtz (Ed.), The humanist alternative: Some definitions of humanism (pp. 98–105). Buffalo, NY: Prometheus Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1974). About behaviorism. New York: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1975a). The shaping of phylogenic behavior. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 35, 409–415.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1975b). The shaping of phylogenic behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 24, 117–120.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1977). Herrnstein and the evolution of behaviorism. American Psychologist, 32, 1006–1012.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1978). Reflections on behaviorism and society. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1979). The shaping of a behaviorist. New York: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1980a). The experimental analysis of operant behavior: A history. In R. W. Reiber & K. Salzinger (Eds.), Psychology: Theoretical-historical perspectives (pp. 191–202). New York: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1980b). The species-specific behavior of ethologists. The Behavior Analyst, 3, 51.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1981). Selection by consequences. Science, 213, 501–504.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1982). Skinner for the classroom (R. Epstein, Ed.). Champaign, IL: Research Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1983a). A better way to deal with selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 377–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1983b). A matter of consequences. New York: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1984a). Author’s response. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 701–707.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1984b). The canonical papers of B. F Skinner. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 473–724.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1984c). The evolution of behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 41, 217–221.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1985). Cognitive science and behaviourism. British Journal of Psychology, 76, 291–301.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1986). The evolution of verbal behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 45, 115–122.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1987). Upon further reflection. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1988). Genes and behavior. In G. Greenberg & E. Tolbach (Eds.), Evolution of social behavior and integrative levels (pp. 77–83). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1989a). The origins of cognitive thought. American Psychologist, 44, 13–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1989b). Recent issues in the analysis of behavior. Columbus, OH: Merrill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1990a). Can psychology be a science of mind? American Psychologist, 45, 1206–1210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1990b, February 5). From behaviorism to teaching machines to enjoying old age. Current Contents: Social and Behavioral Sciences, p. 5.

  • Skinner, B. F (1990c). The non-punitive society. Japanese Journal for Behavior Analysis, 5, 98–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1993). A world of our own. Behaviorology, 1, 3–5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (1999). Cumulative record (Definitive ed., V. G. Laties & A. C. Catania, Eds.). Cambridge, MA: B. F Skinner Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F (2004). Psychology in the year 2000. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 81, 207–213.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F, & Crozier, W. J. (1931). [Review of the book Reflex Action, a Study in the History of Physiological Psychology]. Journal of General Psychology, 5, 125–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F, & Heron, W. T (1937). Effects of caffeine and benzedrine upon conditioning and extinction. The Psychological Record, 1, 340–346.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steven Pinker. (2004, April 26). Time Magazine, p. 114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tiger, L., & Fox, R. (1971). The imperial animal. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, N. (1951). The study of instinct. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Todd, J. T (1987a). The great power of steady misrepresentation: Behaviorism’s presumed denial of instinct. The Behavior Analyst, 10, 117–118.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Todd, J. T (1987b). [Response to Gould and Marler]. Scientific American, 256(4), 4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Todd, J. T, & Morris, E. K. (1992). Case histories in the great power of steady misrepresentation. American Psychologist, 47, 1441– 1453.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Verplanck, W. S. (1955). Since learned behavior is innate, and vice versa, what now? Psychological Review, 62, 139–144.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vicari, E. M. (1929). Mode of inheritance of reaction time and degrees of learning in mice. Journal of Experimental Zoology, 54, 32–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wahlsten, D. (1984). Each behavior is a product of heredity and experience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 699–700.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1924). Behaviorism. New York: Peoples Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. D. (1968). The double helix. New York: Atheneum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. D., & Crick, F (1953, April 25). Molecular structure of nucleic acid. Nature, 171, 137–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M. M. (1991). Are the TEABs coming? [review of The Goal of B. F. Skinner and Behavior Analysis]. Contemporary Psychology, 36, 1085–1086.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yoerg, S. I. (2001). Clever as a fox: Animal intelligence and what it can teach us about ourselves. New York: Bloomsbury.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Edward K. Morris.

Additional information

We thank Katherine L. Stewart for assisting with data collection and David Born for statistical consulting. Earlier versions of this article were presented at the meeting of the Association for Behavior Analysis (Lazo, Morris, & Smith, 2002) and the International Conference of the Brazilian Association for Psychotherapy and Behavioral Medicine and the Association for Behavior Analysis (Morris, Lazo, & Smith, 2004).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Morris, E.K., Lazo, J.F. & Smith, N.G. Whether, when, and why Skinner published on biological participation in behavior. BEHAV ANALYST 27, 153–169 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03393176

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03393176

Key words

Navigation