The Analysis of Verbal Behavior

, Volume 29, Issue 1, pp 109–115 | Cite as

The Relationship Between Punishment History and Skin Conductance Elicited During Swearing

Brief Report

Abstract

Despite its theoretical importance, the effect of past punishment on verbal behavior is often overlooked in research due to the difficulty of measuring it. The present study explores the relationship between physiological arousal in humans and swearing; a behavior likely to have been punished in the typical conditioning history of an individual. Participants’ skin conductance was measured as they read aloud a list of words containing swear words, emotionally salient words, and neutral words. The association between SCR measurements and participants’ scores on questionnaires on previous punishment for swearing was then analyzed. Findings suggest that people have significantly higher physiological arousal when saying swear words than neutral words, and that this arousal is higher for participants who have previously received more frequent punishment for swearing, according to self-report.

Key words

swearing skin conductance physiological arousal punishment classical conditioning 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Dinn, W. M., & Harris, C. L. (2000). Neurocognitive function in antisocial personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 97, 173–190.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Gray, S. G., Hughes, H. H., & Schneider, L. J. (1982). Physiological responsivity to a socially stressful situation: The effect of level of moral development. Psychological Record, 32, 29–34.Google Scholar
  3. Harris, C., Aycicegi, A., & Gleason, J. B. (2003). Taboo words and reprimands elicit greater autonomic reactivity in a first than a second language. Applied Psycholinguistics 24, 561–579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Hill, A. B., & Kemp-Wheeler, S. M. (1989). The influence of context on lexical decision times for emotionally aversive words. Current Psychology: Research and Reviews, 8, 219–227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Jay, T., & Janschewitz, K. (2008). The pragmatics of swearing. Journal of Politeness Research, 4, 267–288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Jay, T., King, K., & Duncan, T. (2006). College students’ memories of punishment for cursing. Sex Roles 32, 123–133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Leech, G., Rayson, P., & Wilson, A. (2001). Word frequencies in written and spoken English: Based on the British National Corpus. London: Longman.Google Scholar
  8. Purkis, H. M., & Lipp, O. V. (2001). Does affective learning exist in the absence of contingency awareness? Learning and Motivation, 32, 84–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Schrauf, R. W. (2000). Bilingual autobiographical memory: Experimental studies and clinical cases. Culture and Psychology, 6, 387–417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York, NY: The Free Press.Google Scholar
  11. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. U.S. Congress, Office of Technology Assessment. (1983). Scientific validity of polygraph testing: A research review and evaluation. Washington, DC: OTA-TM-H-15.Google Scholar
  13. Wischner, G. J., & Gladis, M. T. (1969). Rate of formation of associations by schizophrenic and normal subjects as a function of stimulus or response placement of aversive components. Psychological Reports, 25, 599–605.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Association of Behavior Analysis International 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologySwansea UniversityUSA

Personalised recommendations